Knowledge

Construct validity

Source πŸ“

273:
measurements that are supposed to be unrelated are, in fact, unrelated. Take, for example, a construct of general happiness. If a measure of general happiness had convergent validity, then constructs similar to happiness (satisfaction, contentment, cheerfulness, etc.) should relate positively to the measure of general happiness. If this measure has discriminant validity, then constructs that are not supposed to be related positively to general happiness (sadness, depression, despair, etc.) should not relate to the measure of general happiness. Measures can have one of the subtypes of construct validity and not the other. Using the example of general happiness, a researcher could create an inventory where there is a very high positive correlation between general happiness and contentment, but if there is also a significant positive correlation between happiness and depression, then the measure's construct validity is called into question. The test has convergent validity but not discriminant validity.
305:) are not indicators of intelligence, but volume of the brain is. Removing the theory of phrenology from the nomological net of intelligence and adding the theory of brain mass evolution, constructs of intelligence are made more efficient and more powerful. The weaving of all of these interrelated concepts and their observable traits creates a "net" that supports their theoretical concept. For example, in the nomological network for academic achievement, we would expect observable traits of academic achievement (i.e. GPA, SAT, and ACT scores) to relate to the observable traits for studiousness (hours spent studying, attentiveness in class, detail of notes). If they do not then there is a problem with measurement (of 328:(MTMM) is an approach to examining construct validity developed by Campbell and Fiske (1959). This model examines convergence (evidence that different measurement methods of a construct give similar results) and discriminability (ability to differentiate the construct from other related constructs). It measures six traits: the evaluation of convergent validity, the evaluation of discriminant (divergent) validity, trait-method units, multitrait-multimethods, truly different methodologies, and trait characteristics. This design allows investigators to test for: "convergence across different measures...of the same 'thing'...and for divergence between measures...of related but conceptually distinct 'things'. 73:(1998) have pushed for a unified view of construct validity "...as an integrated evaluative judgment of the degree to which empirical evidence and theoretical rationales support the adequacy and appropriateness of inferences and actions based on test scores..." While Messick's views are popularized in educational measurement and originated in a career around explaining validity in the context of the testing industry, a definition more in line with foundational psychological research, supported by data-driven empirical studies that emphasize statistical and causal reasoning was given by (Borsboom et al., 2004). 116:, etc.). This made it difficult to tell which ones were actually the same and which ones were not useful at all. Until the middle of the 1950s, there were very few universally accepted methods to validate psychological experiments. The main reason for this was because no one had figured out exactly which qualities of the experiments should be looked at before publishing. Between 1950 and 1954 the APA Committee on Psychological Tests met and discussed the issues surrounding the validation of psychological experiments. 366:". Among the questions used around the time of World War I in the battery used to measure intelligence was "In which city do the Dodgers play?" (they were then based in Brooklyn). Recent immigrants to the US from Eastern Europe unfamiliar with the sport of baseball got the answer wrong, and this was used to infer that Eastern Europeans had lower intelligence. The question did not measure intelligence: it only measured how long one had lived in the US and become accultured to a popular pastime. 241:(SEM), and other statistical evaluations. It is important to note that a single study does not prove construct validity. Rather it is a continuous process of evaluation, reevaluation, refinement, and development. Correlations that fit the expected pattern contribute evidence of construct validity. Construct validity is a judgment based on the accumulation of correlations from numerous studies using the instrument being evaluated. 147:
substantive component, structural component, and external component. They are closely related to three stages in the test construction process: constitution of the pool of items, analysis and selection of the internal structure of the pool of items, and correlation of test scores with criteria and other variables.
1058:
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake, & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory (pp. 102βˆ’134).
272:
Convergent and discriminant validity are the two subtypes of validity that make up construct validity. Convergent validity refers to the degree to which two measures of constructs that theoretically should be related, are in fact related. In contrast, discriminant validity tests whether concepts or
228:
Evaluation of construct validity requires that the correlations of the measure be examined in regard to variables that are known to be related to the construct (purportedly measured by the instrument being evaluated or for which there are theoretical grounds for expecting it to be related). This is
291:
defines a construct by illustrating its relation to other constructs and behaviors. It is a representation of the concepts (constructs) of interest in a study, their observable manifestations, and the interrelationship among them. It examines whether the relationships between similar construct are
146:
was that it placed more emphasis on theory as opposed to validation. This emphasis was designed to address a core requirement that validation include some demonstration that the test measures the theoretical construct it purported to measure. Construct validity has three aspects or components: the
91:
Scale purification, i.e. "the process of eliminating items from multi-item scales" (Wieland et al., 2017) can influence construct validity. A framework presented by Wieland et al. (2017) highlights that both statistical and judgmental criteria need to be taken under consideration when making scale
300:
are considered highly related constructs. Through the observation of their underlying components psychologists developed new theoretical constructs such as: controlled attention and short term loading. Creating a nomological net can also make the observation and measurement of existing constructs
309:
or studiousness), or with the purported theory of achievement. If they are indicators of one another then the nomological network, and therefore the constructed theory, of academic achievement is strengthened. Although the nomological network proposed a theory of how to strengthen constructs, it
253:
are yet another method of evaluating construct validity. Intervention studies where a group with low scores in the construct is tested, taught the construct, and then re-measured can demonstrate a test's construct validity. If there is a significant difference pre-test and post-test, which are
150:
In the 1970s there was growing debate between theorists who began to see construct validity as the dominant model pushing towards a more unified theory of validity, and those who continued to work from multiple validity frameworks. Many psychologists and education researchers saw "predictive,
248:
may be utilized. Pilot studies are small scale preliminary studies aimed at testing the feasibility of a full-scale test. These pilot studies establish the strength of their research and allow them to make any necessary adjustments. Another method is the known-groups technique, which involves
127:
in their seminal article "Construct Validity In Psychological Tests". They noted the idea that construct validity was not new at that point; rather, it was a combination of many different types of validity dealing with theoretical concepts. They proposed the following three steps to evaluate
166:
In 1989 Messick presented a new conceptualization of construct validity as a unified and multi-faceted concept. Under this framework, all forms of validity are connected to and are dependent on the quality of the construct. He noted that a unified theory was not his own idea, but rather the
162:
the inter-relatedness of the three different aspects of validity was recognized: "These aspects of validity can be discussed independently, but only for convenience. They are interrelated operationally and logically; only rarely is one of them alone important in a particular situation".
973:
Hammond, K. R., Hamm, R. M., & Grassia, J. (1986). Generalizing over conditions by combining the multitrait multimethod matrix and the representative design of experiments (No. CRJP-255A). Colorado University At Boulder Center For Research On Judgment And
30:
is the accumulation of evidence to support the interpretation of what a measure reflects. Modern validity theory defines construct validity as the overarching concern of validity research, subsuming all other types of validity evidence such as
51:, which is correlated with scores on a given measure (although it is not directly observable). Construct validity examines the question: Does the measure behave like the theory says a measure of that construct should behave? 376:
experimental designs should be used where possible. That is, the evaluator of a particular participant should be unaware of what intervention has been performed on that particular participant or should be independent of the
351:
brightening the ambient light levels improved worker productivity. They eventually determined the basis for this paradoxical result: workers who were aware of being observed worked harder no matter what the change in the
233:(MTMM) of examining construct validity described in Campbell and Fiske's landmark paper (1959). There are other methods to evaluate construct validity besides MTMM. It can be evaluated through different forms of 42:
Construct validity is the appropriateness of inferences made on the basis of observations or measurements (often test scores), specifically whether a test can reasonably be considered to reflect the intended
1130:
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, L. L. (2006). An introduction to communication in the classroom: The role of communication in teaching and training. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
249:
administering the measurement instrument to groups expected to differ due to known characteristics. Hypothesized relationship testing involves logical analysis based on theory or prior research.
167:
culmination of debate and discussion within the scientific community over the preceding decades. There are six aspects of construct validity in Messick's unified theory of construct validity:
292:
considered with relationships between the observed measures of the constructs. A thorough observation of constructs relationships to each other it can generate new constructs. For example,
635:
Polit DF Beck CT (2012). Nursing Research: Generating and Assessing Evidence for Nursing Practice, 9th ed. Philadelphia, USA: Wolters Klower Health, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
100:
Throughout the 1940s scientists had been trying to come up with ways to validate experiments prior to publishing them. The result of this was a plethora of different validities (
822:
Wieland, A., Durach, C.F., Kembro, J. & Treiblmaier, H. (2017), Statistical and judgmental criteria for scale purification, Supply Chain Management, Vol. 22, No. 4,
645:
Messick, S. (1995). "Validity of psychological assessment: Validation of inferences from persons' responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning".
1069:
Ackerman P. L.; Beier M. E.; Boyle M. O. (2002). "Individual differences in working memory within a nomological network of cognitive and perceptual speed abilities".
672:
Schotte, C. K. W.; Maes, M.; Cluydts, R.; De Doncker, D.; Cosyns, P. (1997). "Construct validity of the Beck Depression Inventory in a depressive population".
250: 113: 101: 287:
Lee Cronbach and Paul Meehl (1955) proposed that the development of a nomological net was essential to the measurement of a test's construct validity. A
158: 1242: 347:: in a 1925 industrial ergonomics study conducted at the Hawthorne Works factory outside Chicago, experimenters observed that both lowering 208:
How construct validity should properly be viewed is still a subject of debate for validity theorists. The core of the difference lies in an
76:
Key to construct validity are the theoretical ideas behind the trait under consideration, i.e. the concepts that organize how aspects of
174:– What are the potential risks if the scores are invalid or inappropriately interpreted? Is the test still worthwhile given the risks? 88:
states that, "The best construct is the one around which we can build the greatest number of inferences, in the most direct fashion."
372:
may be communicated unintentionally to the participants non-verbally, eliciting the desired effect. To control for this possibility,
54:
Construct validity is essential to the perceived overall validity of the test. Construct validity is particularly important in the
1022:
Peter, J. P. (1981). Construct validity: a review of basic issues and marketing practices. Journal of Marketing Research, 133-145.
785:
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2004). The Concept of Validity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 1061–1071.
807: 336:
Apparent construct validity can be misleading due to a range of problems in hypothesis formulation and experimental design.
395:(covariates): The root cause for the observed effects may be due to variables that have not been considered or measured. 343:: If the participant knows, or guesses, the desired end-result, the participant's actions may change. An example is the 192:– Do the interrelationships of dimensions measured by the test correlate with the construct of interest and test scores? 44: 408: 1262: 325: 319: 230: 238: 433: 301:
more efficient by pinpointing errors. Researchers have found that studying the bumps on the human skull (
47:. Constructs are abstractions that are deliberately created by researchers in order to conceptualize the 155:, construct validity was the whole of validity from a scientific point of view" In the 1974 version of 939:
Campbell D. T. (1959). "Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix".
838:
Loevinger J (1957). "Objective Tests As Instruments Of Psychological Theory: Monograph Supplement 9".
907:
Messick, S. (1995). "Standards of validity and the validity of standards in performance assessment".
1163: 709: 448: 1158: 1032:
Dimitrov D. M.; Rumrill Jr P. D. (2003). "Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of change".
1139:
Gould, S. J. (1996). The Mismeasure of Man. 2nd edition. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
363: 267: 244:
Most researchers attempt to test the construct validity before the main research. To do this
1104:
Edgington, E. S. (1974). "A new tabulation of statistical procedures used in APA journals".
306: 8: 418: 288: 282: 263: 1247: 1226: 1176: 920: 855: 768: 530: 492: 36: 23: 685: 399:
An in-depth exploration of the threats to construct validity is presented in Trochim.
1086: 1041: 1005: 985: 956: 859: 803: 772: 689: 522: 428: 413: 359: 1207: 1180: 1168: 1113: 1078: 997: 948: 916: 896:(3rd ed.). New York: American Council on Education/Macmillan. pp. 13–103. 847: 760: 718: 681: 654: 579: 534: 512: 504: 443: 423: 384: 344: 109: 32: 600: 387:
to measure happiness will exclude relevant information from outside the workplace.
254:
analyzed by statistical tests, then this may demonstrate good construct validity.
453: 234: 55: 48: 1172: 1082: 1001: 786: 722: 658: 297: 217: 70: 1212: 1195: 764: 1256: 851: 584: 567: 458: 438: 245: 143: 135:
developing ways to measure the hypothetical constructs proposed by the theory
105: 63: 1149:
MacKenzie S. B. (2003). "The dangers of poor construct conceptualization".
1090: 1045: 1009: 960: 823: 526: 373: 293: 209: 186:– Is the theoretical foundation underlying the construct of interest sound? 142:
Many psychologists noted that an important role of construct validation in
124: 81: 693: 391: 77: 24:
indicators represent or reflect a concept that is not directly measurable
517: 198:– Does the test have convergent, discriminant, and predictive qualities? 1229:, Trochim, William M. The Research Methods Knowledge Base, 2nd edition. 302: 213: 204:– Does the test generalize across different groups, settings and tasks? 120: 85: 59: 1117: 952: 508: 310:
doesn't tell us how we can assess the construct validity in a study.
1068: 132:
articulating a set of theoretical concepts and their interrelations
751:
Messick, Samuel (1998). "Test validity: A matter of consequence".
358:(intentional or unintentional). An example of this is provided in 617: 565: 180:– Do test items appear to be measuring the construct of interest? 119:
Around this time the term construct validity was first coined by
1196:"Construction of confounding plans for mixed factorial designs" 1031: 707:
Guion, R. M. (1980). "On trinitarian doctrines of validity".
1034:
Work: A Journal of Prevention, Assessment and Rehabilitation
671: 1248:
Provides a visual representation of the nomological network
983: 550: 1193: 546: 544: 257: 892:
Messick, S. (1989). "Validity.". In R. L. Linn (ed.).
986:"Quantifying construct validity: Two simple measures" 541: 1220: 159:
Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing
151:concurrent, and content validities as essentially 1254: 1103: 740:. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents. 622:. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents. 331: 313: 1148: 938: 138:empirically testing the hypothesized relations 490: 990:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 909:Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice 833: 831: 787:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1061 568:"Construct validity in software engineering" 566:SjΓΈberg, D. I. K.; Bergersen, G. R. (2022). 486: 484: 482: 480: 478: 476: 474: 1151:Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 1071:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 934: 932: 930: 837: 493:"Construct validity in psychological tests" 797: 791: 638: 602:Interpretation of educational measurements 1243:Useful reference guide for research terms 1211: 1162: 967: 900: 885: 828: 583: 572:IEEE Transactions on Software Engineering 516: 471: 927: 824:https://doi.org/10.1108/SCM-07-2016-0230 700: 665: 906: 891: 750: 744: 729: 644: 631: 629: 381:Defining predicted outcome too narrowly 16:Measure of indicator representativeness 1255: 1059:Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 984:Westen Drew; Rosenthal Robert (2003). 866: 598: 491:Cronbach, L. J.; Meehl, P. E. (1955). 276: 1200:The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 735: 706: 615: 872: 626: 258:Convergent and discriminant validity 551:Cook T. D.; Campbell D. T. (1979). 13: 1194:White D.; Hultquist R. A. (1965). 921:10.1111/j.1745-3992.1995.tb00881.x 873:Kane, M. T. (2006). "Validation". 14: 1274: 1236: 1187: 1142: 1133: 1124: 1097: 1062: 1052: 1025: 1016: 977: 816: 779: 409:Statistical conclusion validity 674:Journal of Affective Disorders 609: 592: 559: 1: 1227:Threats to Construct Validity 686:10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00094-3 464: 332:Threats to construct validity 326:multitrait-multimethod matrix 320:Multitrait-multimethod matrix 314:Multitrait-multimethod matrix 231:multitrait-multimethod matrix 223: 738:Testing in language programs 619:Testing in language programs 239:structural equation modeling 7: 1173:10.1177/0092070303031003011 1083:10.1037/0096-3445.131.4.567 798:Pennington, Donald (2003). 599:Kelley, Truman Lee (1927). 555:. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 434:Reliability (psychometrics) 402: 383:. For instance, using only 356:Bias in experimental design 22:concerns how well a set of 10: 1279: 1002:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.608 753:Social Indicators Research 723:10.1037/0735-7028.11.3.385 659:10.1037/0003-066x.50.9.741 317: 280: 261: 95: 852:10.2466/pr0.1957.3.3.635 585:10.1109/TSE.2022.3176725 92:purification decisions. 1213:10.1214/aoms/1177699997 894:Educational Measurement 875:Educational Measurement 765:10.1023/a:1006964925094 710:Professional Psychology 605:. New York: World Book. 449:Jingle-jangle fallacies 370:Researcher expectations 941:Psychological Bulletin 497:Psychological Bulletin 69:Psychologists such as 66:and language studies. 1263:Validity (statistics) 1106:American Psychologist 840:Psychological Reports 800:Essential Personality 736:Brown, J. D. (1996). 647:American Psychologist 616:Brown, J. D. (1996). 553:Quasi-experimentation 392:Confounding variables 364:The Mismeasure of Man 268:discriminant validity 307:academic achievement 251:Intervention studies 229:consistent with the 128:construct validity: 28:Construct validation 419:Ecological validity 341:Hypothesis guessing 289:nomological network 283:nomological network 277:Nomological network 264:convergent validity 212:difference between 84:, etc. are viewed. 114:empirical validity 102:intrinsic validity 37:criterion validity 20:Construct validity 809:978-0-340-76118-2 429:External validity 414:Internal validity 360:Stephen Jay Gould 1270: 1230: 1224: 1218: 1217: 1215: 1206:(4): 1256–1271. 1191: 1185: 1184: 1166: 1146: 1140: 1137: 1131: 1128: 1122: 1121: 1118:10.1037/h0035846 1101: 1095: 1094: 1066: 1060: 1056: 1050: 1049: 1029: 1023: 1020: 1014: 1013: 981: 975: 971: 965: 964: 953:10.1037/h0046016 936: 925: 924: 904: 898: 897: 889: 883: 882: 870: 864: 863: 835: 826: 820: 814: 813: 795: 789: 783: 777: 776: 748: 742: 741: 733: 727: 726: 704: 698: 697: 669: 663: 662: 642: 636: 633: 624: 623: 613: 607: 606: 596: 590: 589: 587: 578:(3): 1374–1396. 563: 557: 556: 548: 539: 538: 520: 509:10.1037/h0040957 488: 444:Logical validity 424:Content validity 385:job satisfaction 345:Hawthorne effect 202:Generalizability 110:logical validity 33:content validity 1278: 1277: 1273: 1272: 1271: 1269: 1268: 1267: 1253: 1252: 1239: 1234: 1233: 1225: 1221: 1192: 1188: 1164:10.1.1.417.7311 1147: 1143: 1138: 1134: 1129: 1125: 1102: 1098: 1067: 1063: 1057: 1053: 1030: 1026: 1021: 1017: 982: 978: 972: 968: 937: 928: 905: 901: 890: 886: 871: 867: 836: 829: 821: 817: 810: 796: 792: 784: 780: 749: 745: 734: 730: 705: 701: 670: 666: 643: 639: 634: 627: 614: 610: 597: 593: 564: 560: 549: 542: 489: 472: 467: 454:Lee J. Cronbach 405: 334: 322: 316: 285: 279: 270: 262:Main articles: 260: 235:factor analysis 226: 210:epistemological 98: 56:social sciences 49:latent variable 17: 12: 11: 5: 1276: 1266: 1265: 1251: 1250: 1245: 1238: 1237:External links 1235: 1232: 1231: 1219: 1186: 1157:(3): 323–326. 1141: 1132: 1123: 1096: 1077:(4): 567–589. 1061: 1051: 1040:(2): 159–165. 1024: 1015: 996:(3): 608–618. 976: 966: 926: 899: 884: 865: 846:(3): 635–694. 827: 815: 808: 790: 778: 759:(1–3): 35–44. 743: 728: 717:(3): 385–398. 699: 680:(2): 115–125. 664: 653:(9): 741–749. 637: 625: 608: 591: 558: 540: 503:(4): 281–302. 469: 468: 466: 463: 462: 461: 456: 451: 446: 441: 436: 431: 426: 421: 416: 411: 404: 401: 397: 396: 388: 378: 367: 362:'s 1981 book " 353: 333: 330: 318:Main article: 315: 312: 298:working memory 281:Main article: 278: 275: 259: 256: 225: 222: 218:postpositivist 206: 205: 199: 193: 187: 181: 175: 140: 139: 136: 133: 97: 94: 71:Samuel Messick 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1275: 1264: 1261: 1260: 1258: 1249: 1246: 1244: 1241: 1240: 1228: 1223: 1214: 1209: 1205: 1201: 1197: 1190: 1182: 1178: 1174: 1170: 1165: 1160: 1156: 1152: 1145: 1136: 1127: 1119: 1115: 1111: 1107: 1100: 1092: 1088: 1084: 1080: 1076: 1072: 1065: 1055: 1047: 1043: 1039: 1035: 1028: 1019: 1011: 1007: 1003: 999: 995: 991: 987: 980: 970: 962: 958: 954: 950: 947:(2): 81–105. 946: 942: 935: 933: 931: 922: 918: 914: 910: 903: 895: 888: 880: 876: 869: 861: 857: 853: 849: 845: 841: 834: 832: 825: 819: 811: 805: 801: 794: 788: 782: 774: 770: 766: 762: 758: 754: 747: 739: 732: 724: 720: 716: 712: 711: 703: 695: 691: 687: 683: 679: 675: 668: 660: 656: 652: 648: 641: 632: 630: 621: 620: 612: 604: 603: 595: 586: 581: 577: 573: 569: 562: 554: 547: 545: 536: 532: 528: 524: 519: 514: 510: 506: 502: 498: 494: 487: 485: 483: 481: 479: 477: 475: 470: 460: 459:Paul E. Meehl 457: 455: 452: 450: 447: 445: 442: 440: 439:Face validity 437: 435: 432: 430: 427: 425: 422: 420: 417: 415: 412: 410: 407: 406: 400: 394: 393: 389: 386: 382: 379: 377:experimenter. 375: 371: 368: 365: 361: 357: 354: 350: 346: 342: 339: 338: 337: 329: 327: 321: 311: 308: 304: 299: 295: 290: 284: 274: 269: 265: 255: 252: 247: 246:pilot studies 242: 240: 236: 232: 221: 219: 215: 211: 203: 200: 197: 194: 191: 188: 185: 182: 179: 176: 173: 172:Consequential 170: 169: 168: 164: 161: 160: 154: 148: 145: 144:psychometrics 137: 134: 131: 130: 129: 126: 122: 117: 115: 111: 107: 106:face validity 103: 93: 89: 87: 83: 79: 74: 72: 67: 65: 64:psychometrics 61: 57: 52: 50: 46: 40: 38: 34: 29: 25: 21: 1222: 1203: 1199: 1189: 1154: 1150: 1144: 1135: 1126: 1109: 1105: 1099: 1074: 1070: 1064: 1054: 1037: 1033: 1027: 1018: 993: 989: 979: 969: 944: 940: 912: 908: 902: 893: 887: 878: 874: 868: 843: 839: 818: 799: 793: 781: 756: 752: 746: 737: 731: 714: 708: 702: 677: 673: 667: 650: 646: 640: 618: 611: 601: 594: 575: 571: 561: 552: 518:11299/184279 500: 496: 398: 390: 380: 374:double-blind 369: 355: 352:environment. 348: 340: 335: 323: 294:intelligence 286: 271: 243: 227: 207: 201: 195: 189: 183: 177: 171: 165: 156: 152: 149: 141: 125:Lee Cronbach 118: 99: 90: 82:intelligence 75: 68: 53: 41: 27: 19: 18: 220:theorists. 184:Substantive 78:personality 915:(4): 5–8. 802:. Arnold. 465:References 303:phrenology 224:Evaluation 214:positivist 190:Structural 121:Paul Meehl 86:Paul Meehl 60:psychology 1159:CiteSeerX 860:145640521 773:142684085 45:construct 1257:Category 1091:12500864 1046:12671209 1010:12635920 961:13634291 881:: 17–64. 527:13245896 403:See also 196:External 1181:5930358 974:Policy. 694:9479615 535:5312179 178:Content 96:History 1179:  1161:  1112:: 61. 1089:  1044:  1008:  959:  858:  806:  771:  692:  533:  525:  153:ad hoc 1177:S2CID 856:S2CID 769:S2CID 531:S2CID 1087:PMID 1042:PMID 1006:PMID 957:PMID 804:ISBN 690:PMID 523:PMID 324:The 296:and 266:and 216:and 157:The 123:and 35:and 1208:doi 1169:doi 1114:doi 1079:doi 1075:131 998:doi 949:doi 917:doi 848:doi 761:doi 719:doi 682:doi 655:doi 580:doi 513:hdl 505:doi 349:and 1259:: 1204:36 1202:. 1198:. 1175:. 1167:. 1155:31 1153:. 1110:29 1108:. 1085:. 1073:. 1038:20 1036:. 1004:. 994:84 992:. 988:. 955:. 945:56 943:. 929:^ 913:14 911:. 877:. 854:. 842:. 830:^ 767:. 757:45 755:. 715:11 713:. 688:. 678:46 676:. 651:50 649:. 628:^ 576:49 574:. 570:. 543:^ 529:. 521:. 511:. 501:52 499:. 495:. 473:^ 237:, 112:, 108:, 104:, 80:, 62:, 58:, 39:. 26:. 1216:. 1210:: 1183:. 1171:: 1120:. 1116:: 1093:. 1081:: 1048:. 1012:. 1000:: 963:. 951:: 923:. 919:: 879:4 862:. 850:: 844:3 812:. 775:. 763:: 725:. 721:: 696:. 684:: 661:. 657:: 588:. 582:: 537:. 515:: 507::

Index

indicators represent or reflect a concept that is not directly measurable
content validity
criterion validity
construct
latent variable
social sciences
psychology
psychometrics
Samuel Messick
personality
intelligence
Paul Meehl
intrinsic validity
face validity
logical validity
empirical validity
Paul Meehl
Lee Cronbach
psychometrics
Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing
epistemological
positivist
postpositivist
multitrait-multimethod matrix
factor analysis
structural equation modeling
pilot studies
Intervention studies
convergent validity
discriminant validity

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑