Knowledge

Vigilance (behavioural ecology)

Source 📝

98: 355: 498: 436:) in large groups both spend less time with their head up than individuals in small groups; however, collective vigilance increases with group size in ostriches but not rheas. Therefore, the many eyes hypothesis does not always hold. Although individuals in groups may be able to decrease the time they spend scanning, and hence reach their energy requirements in a shorter period of time, the increased competition for food in groups may mean that animals need to devote more time overall to foraging due to an increased allocation of time for 216:) is present, smaller sunfish spend the majority of their time foraging in the reeds despite this choice reducing their food intake and seasonal growth rate. Sunfish that are too large to be eaten by the bass forage almost entirely on benthos. Although staying in the reeds results in a slower growth rate and a longer period of being a size vulnerable to predators, for maximum survival sunfish choose to remain in the reeds feeding on plankton until they reach a certain size and then leave to feed on benthos. 120:) alter their behaviour according to the relative costs and benefits when foraging in the open. Small food items are consumed immediately to maximise energy intake, as they require little handling time so the predation risk is low. Large items that require a long handling time, and hence time exposed to predators, are carried back to the safety of a tree to minimise predation risk. Although there is an energetic cost to transporting food, large food items have a high contribution to 571:
detection of the group while being able to feed more than other individuals. However, cheating is not a stable strategy because if all members of the group did it, then there would be no collective vigilance. Adopting a very high level of vigilance in a group is also not a stable strategy as an individual who adopts a lower level of vigilance would have a greater advantage. The evolutionarily stable (
172:. The cost to this choice is that the sticklebacks must concentrate on picking out the prey due to the 'predator confusion effect' where many moving targets make it difficult for predators to pick out individual prey. This choice means that the sticklebacks are less able to scan for predators; however, the risk of starvation is relatively higher than the risk of predation. Similarly, 420:
decrease the time it allocates to vigilance without any increase in its personal risk of being attacked - particularly as large groups already have a diluted predation risk. Lower individual levels of vigilance with increasing group size has been observed in many bird, mammal and fish species. Individual
578:
Maintaining individual vigilance may be a more beneficial strategy if vigilant animals gain some sort of advantage. Non-vigilant individuals are often the last to flee to safety, as groups generally flee in succession from the individual who spots the predator, individuals who were vigilant when the
513:
In some species, individuals in a foraging group take on sentry duty. The sentinels look out for predators (often from a good vantage point) while the rest of the group forages, and sound an alarm when they spot a predator. Sentry duty is particularly important for species whose foraging activity is
474:
of birds often exhibit collective detection. One or more birds initially detect the threat, and other birds that did not perceive the threat detect their departure and also respond by fleeing. The departure of multiple birds simultaneously is likely to be a more effective alarm signal than that of a
147:
Another factor that influences vigilance is the benefit that is expected from foraging in the absence of predation. This is dependent on the quality of the food as well as the energetic state of the individual. If there is much to be gained from feeding, foragers may forgo vigilance. Similarly, if
304:
A group of animals may be better at both finding and capturing food than solitary animals. For species that feed on food that is patchy, the likelihood of locating food is greater if individuals search for food independently and inform the rest of the group when a food patch is found. Information
419:
Larger groups may have a greater overall vigilance than small groups due to the ‘many eyes’ hypothesis: more eyes scanning for predators means that the proportion of time that at least one individual is vigilant (collective vigilance) is higher. This improved vigilance may allow an individual to
467:
in successful attacks, early detection of predators reduces predation risk. The detection of a predator by one individual only translates to collective detection if that individual makes some sort of signal to alert the rest of the group. The signal may be a deliberate call made by the vigilant
570:
In a large group, it is possible for an individual to cheat by scanning less than other members of the group without having any negative effect on the collective vigilance of the group. Cheating would seem to be the most beneficial strategy as the individual still benefits from the collective
188:
between foraging, body reserves and predation risk. The feedback can influence an individual's choice of where, when and what to feed on. If the predation risk is so great that an animal must maintain a level of vigilance that drastically inhibits feeding, it may opt for an alternative.
552:
as an individual on sentry duty is unable to feed, may be more exposed to predators, and may attract the predators' attention when they make an alarm call. However, meerkats on sentry duty are at no greater risk of predation as they are generally the first to detect predators (e.g.
135:
the forager, as well as the likelihood that the forager can escape the predator if it is not vigilant. Animals prioritise vigilance over feeding when the predation risk is high. For example, yellow-eyed juncos spend more time scanning for predators when a potential predator, a
561:
species) and flee to safety. Meerkats also only go on guard once they are satiated, so if no other individual is on sentry duty, guarding may be the most beneficial behaviour as the individual has no requirement for feeding and can benefit from early predator detection.
234:
without husks, for example, require little handling by birds so are rapidly pecked up with the bird's head down, which is incompatible with vigilance. In situations of high predation risk, animals may choose foods that can be foraged while maintaining vigilance. When
373:
to capture prey - for example, hawks are unable to capture more than one yellow-eyed junco per attack. Large groups also cause predator confusion as it makes it difficult for prey to focus on one target. Groups of animals may engage in communal defences, such as
229:
A raised head is the most common indicator of vigilance, as many animals require their heads to be lowered to search for and handle food. Different foods require different handling that can affect the amount of vigilance an animal can maintain.
267:
foraging come with a variety of costs and benefits, but for many animals, foraging in a group is the most optimal strategy. Among the many benefits of group foraging, reduction of predation risk by improved vigilance is one.
251:), large pieces of food are more beneficial as they require a longer handling time that can be simultaneously spent scanning, whereas birds feeding on small pieces must intermittently stop foraging to scan their environment. 489:: individuals at the edge of the flotilla detect a predator and move, bumping into their neighbours, who in turn start moving and bumping into more individuals. This ‘wave’ of alarm has been termed the ‘Trafalgar Effect’. 280:
because they are able to spot them and fly away more quickly than they would individually. This is because in larger flocks, it is more likely that one bird will notice the hawk sooner and alert the group by flying away.
79:
and safety from predation. As time allocated to scanning reduces the time spent feeding, vigilant individuals must devote more time to foraging to obtain the required food intake. This impedes other activities, such as
462:
Large groups of animals are able to detect predators earlier because of the higher probability that at least one individual is being vigilant when the predator approaches. As many predators rely on the element of
386:
Dilution of predation risk only occurs if groups of animals are not more prone to attack than individuals. Often large groups are more conspicuous to predators so grouping may increase the risk of attack.
66:
may be used to alert the group to the presence of predators. Groups of some species have at least one individual on sentry duty who looks out for predators on a perch while the rest of the group forages.
845:
Krause, J. & Godin, J.J. (1995) Predator preferences for attacking particular prey group sizes: consequences for predator hunting success and prey predation risk. Animal Behaviour 50: 465-473
579:
first animal departs, and finally the non-vigilant individuals. Predators may also target less vigilant prey as they are likely to detect the predator later and therefore respond more slowly.
951:
Clutton-Brock, T.H., O’Riain, M.J., Brotherton, P.N.M., Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A.S. & Manser, M. (1999) Selfish sentinels in cooperative mammals. Science 284: 1640-1644
407:) to attack based on how visually conspicuous they are, preferring shoals that are larger or exhibit more movement. Foraging in groups requires sharing so may also lead to greater 930:
Wickler, W. (1985) Coordination of vigilance in bird groups, the ‘Watchman’s Song’ hypothesis. Zeitschrift fur Tierphysiologie, Tierernahrung und Futtermittelkunde 69: 250-253
537:
Often, the sentry makes quiet calls that function as a ‘watchman’s song’ to reassure the rest of the group that an individual is on guard. In response to a vocalising sentry,
247:, they prefer to feed on larger pieces of food than when they are part of a larger flock. As individuals in smaller flocks have a greater need to be vigilant (see more in 84:, and prolongs their exposure to predation as foraging occurs away from shelter. When foraging time is limited, vigilant animals are left with a reduced energy intake. 912:
Treherne, J.E. & Foster, W.A. (1981) Group transmission of predator avoidance behaviour in a marine insect: the Trafalgar Effect. Animal Behaviour 29: 911-917
866:
Fernandez, G.J., Capurro, A.F. & Reboreda, J.C. (2003) Effect of group size on individual and collective vigilance in greater rheas. Ethology 109: 413-425
31:
as animals must often venture away from the safety of shelter to find food. However, being vigilant comes at the expense of time spent feeding, so there is a
369:
an individual's risk of being attacked, as the more group members there are, the lower the probability that that individual will be the victim. Grouping may
921:
Rasa, O.A.E. (1986) Coordinated vigilance in dwarf mongoose family groups: the ‘Watchman’s Song’ hypothesis and the costs of guarding. Ethology 71: 340-344
90:
can be used to predict foraging decisions of an animal based on costs (predation risk, starvation) and benefits (safety, food), which are also affected by
854:
Lima, S.L. & Dill, L.M. (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619-640
836:
Foster, W.A. & Treherne, J.E. (1981) Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect. Nature 293: 466-467
575:) scanning rate is the rate that if all group members adopted, an individual that scanned more or less frequently would have a lower chance of survival. 75:
Vigilance and feeding (both searching for and handling food) are generally mutually exclusive activities, leading to foragers facing a trade-off between
990:
Fitzgibbon, C.D. (1989) A cost to individuals with reduced vigilance in groups of Thomson’s gazelles hunted by cheetahs. Animal Behaviour 37(3): 508-510
939:
Hollen, L.I., Bell, M.B.V. & Radford, A.N. (2008) Cooperative sentinel calling? Foragers gain increased biomass intake. Current Biology 18: 576-579
176:
that have been deprived of food exhibit lower levels of vigilance, instead focusing on rapid feeding, which is a behaviour incompatible with scanning.
818:
Clark, C.W. & Mangel, M. (1984) Foraging and flocking strategies: information in an uncertain environment. The American Naturalist 123(5): 626-641
693:
Lima, S.L., Valone, T.J. & Caraco, T. (1985) Foraging-efficiency – predation-risk trade-off in the grey squirrel. Animal Behaviour 33: 155-165
545:) decrease their own vigilance, spread out further from the group and forage in more exposed patches resulting in a higher intake of biomass. 972:
McNamara, J.M. & Houston, A.I. (1992) Evolutionarily stable levels of vigilance as a function of group size. Animal Behaviour 43: 641-658
248: 705:
Brown, J.S. (1999) Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evolutionary Ecology Research 1: 49-71
351:
in groups of prey, often causing them to flee in different directions making it easier for the predators to single out a target.
809:
Lima, S.L. (1988) Vigilance and diet selection: a simple example in the dark-eyed junco. Canadian Journal of Zoology 66: 593-596
827:
Stander, P.E. (1992) Cooperative hunting in lions: the role of the individual. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 29: 445-454
663:
Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. & West, S.A. (2012) An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology 4th edn, Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, UK
963:
Pulliam, H.R. (1982) The scanning behavior of juncos: a game-theoretical approach. Journal of Theoretical Biology 95: 89-103
765:
Gilliam, J.F. (1982) Foraging under mortality risk in size-structured populations. PhD thesis. Michigan State University, MI
35:
between the two. The length of time animals devote to vigilance is dependent on many factors including predation risk and
305:
sharing in this way has little cost to the individual who locates the food if it occurs in high abundance in the patch.
717:
Caraco, T., Martindale, S. & Pulliam, H.R. (1980) Avian flocking in the presence of a predator. Nature 285: 400-401
294: 97: 751:
Lima, S.L. (1995) Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: the group-size effect. Animal Behaviour 49: 11-20
366: 468:
individual (as in the case of meerkats) or simply the departure of the individual who has detected the predator.
731:
Milinski, M. & Heller, R. (1978) Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of sticklebacks (
311:
allows predators to take down larger prey, as well as prey that can outrun the predator but can be caught by an
677:
Illius, A.W. & Fitzgibbon, C. (1994) Costs of vigilance in foraging ungulates. Animal Behaviour 47: 481-484
572: 896:
Barnard, C.J. (1980) Factors affecting flock size mean and variance in a winter population of house sparrows (
156:
than from predation, it is more beneficial to sacrifice vigilance to fulfill their energy requirements. When
878:
Roberts, G. (1996) Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Animal Behaviour 51: 1077-086
514:
incompatible with vigilance, or who feed in areas where they are highly exposed to predators. For example,
1005: 619: 149: 36: 624: 375: 981:
Lima, S.L. (1994) On the personal benefits of anti-predatory vigilance. Animal Behaviour 48: 734-736
609: 157: 774:
Clarke, J.A. (1983) Moonlight’s influence on predator/prey interactions between short-eyed owls (
604: 62:, and allows them to reduce their own vigilance while the vigilance of the group is maintained. 396: 324: 538: 515: 113: 102: 588: 486: 222:
alter the timing of their foraging based on the level of light – avoiding feeding when the
887:
Bertram, B.C.R. (1980) Vigilance and group size in ostriches. Animal Behaviour 28: 278-286
354: 8: 791:
Makowska, J. & Kramer, D.L. (2007) Vigilance during food handling in grey squirrels,
308: 184:
The state of an animal can change due to its behaviour and vice versa due to the dynamic
128: 59: 23:, refers to an animal's monitoring of its surroundings in order to heighten awareness of 475:
single bird as birds regularly depart flocks for reasons other than predator detection.
109:) feeding in the safety of a tree to minimise exposure to predators during food handling 549: 464: 370: 344: 269: 165: 20: 527: 358: 260: 43: 137: 87: 290: 264: 497: 236: 213: 365:
Foraging in a group also has some anti-predator benefits. Being part of a group
132: 999: 614: 471: 441: 244: 457: 429: 219: 205: 63: 526:
from the ground to eat; an activity which requires fixation of both their
485:) transmit predator avoidance behaviour to the group through the sense of 408: 121: 437: 388: 336: 153: 91: 42:
Vigilance is often observed in animals that forage in groups, such as
531: 523: 223: 169: 164:) are deprived of food, they prefer to feed in locations with a high 76: 32: 24: 478: 201: 193: 185: 28: 580: 502: 421: 277: 209: 51: 554: 312: 81: 558: 400: 348: 328: 212:
which are a better quality food source. When a predator (the
173: 440:
during foraging. For example, the rate of fighting between
347:
in a formation. The presence of many predators also causes
316: 231: 595:) before initiating a chase and target them for attack. 276:) in large flocks are more able to escape predation by 70: 226:
is bright as this is when predation risk is highest.
27:
presence. Vigilance is an important behaviour during
509:) on sentry duty looks out for predators on a perch 144:), is present compared to when the hawk is absent. 782:). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 13: 205-209 295:Anti-predator adaptation § Safety in numbers 997: 127:The overall predation risk is a function of the 862: 860: 908: 906: 805: 803: 801: 152:animals have a higher chance of dying from 959: 957: 947: 945: 874: 872: 747: 745: 743: 741: 727: 725: 723: 689: 687: 685: 683: 179: 857: 761: 759: 757: 659: 343:), with each lioness adopting particular 60:dilutes an individual's risk of predation 903: 848: 798: 713: 711: 673: 671: 669: 657: 655: 653: 651: 649: 647: 645: 643: 641: 639: 496: 448:) increases with the size of the flock. 353: 96: 954: 942: 881: 869: 738: 720: 701: 699: 680: 414: 131:, activity and ability of predators to 998: 839: 830: 754: 361:form huge flocks of thousands of birds 254: 812: 768: 708: 666: 636: 124:, so the benefits outweigh the cost. 696: 71:Feeding and predation risk trade-off 13: 14: 1017: 284: 451: 381: 200:) has the choice of foraging on 984: 975: 966: 933: 924: 915: 890: 821: 785: 795:. Animal Behaviour 74: 153-158 291:Foraging § Group foraging 1: 630: 548:Guarding may appear to be an 371:swamp the predator's capacity 299: 492: 7: 620:Collective animal behaviour 598: 565: 10: 1022: 455: 378:, for further protection. 288: 900:) Behaviour 74(1):114-127 625:Elk antipredator behavior 158:three-spined sticklebacks 610:Anti-predator adaptation 605:Optimal foraging theory 587:) select less vigilant 327:for large prey such as 180:Habitat and food choice 94:such as hunger levels. 780:Peromyscus maniculatus 735:). Nature 275: 642-644 733:Gasterosteus aculeatus 510: 362: 162:Gasterosteus aculeatus 110: 58:). Foraging in groups 500: 456:Further information: 357: 341:Connochaetes taurinus 289:Further information: 210:benthic invertebrates 100: 898:Passer domesticus L. 793:Sciurus carolinensis 550:altruistic behaviour 438:aggressive behaviour 415:Effect of group size 142:Parabuteo unicinctus 118:Sciurus carolinensis 107:Sciurus carolinensis 405:Poecilia reticulate 255:Vigilance in groups 249:Vigilance in groups 198:Lepomis macrochirus 21:behavioural ecology 1006:Behavioral ecology 593:Eudorcas thomsonii 589:Thomson's gazelles 511: 507:Suricata suricatta 483:Halobates robustus 363: 359:Red-billed queleas 111: 56:Suricata suricatta 44:yellow-eyed juncos 19:, in the field of 543:Turdoides bicolor 446:Passer domesticus 393:Aequidens pulcher 220:Nocturnal animals 204:in the safety of 192:For example, the 88:Optimality models 1013: 991: 988: 982: 979: 973: 970: 964: 961: 952: 949: 940: 937: 931: 928: 922: 919: 913: 910: 901: 894: 888: 885: 879: 876: 867: 864: 855: 852: 846: 843: 837: 834: 828: 825: 819: 816: 810: 807: 796: 789: 783: 778:) and deermice ( 772: 766: 763: 752: 749: 736: 729: 718: 715: 706: 703: 694: 691: 678: 675: 664: 661: 585:Acinonyx jubatus 520:Helogale parvula 426:Struthio camelus 274:Columba palumbus 243:) feed in small 237:dark-eyed juncos 194:bluegill sunfish 48:Junco phaeonutus 1021: 1020: 1016: 1015: 1014: 1012: 1011: 1010: 996: 995: 994: 989: 985: 980: 976: 971: 967: 962: 955: 950: 943: 938: 934: 929: 925: 920: 916: 911: 904: 895: 891: 886: 882: 877: 870: 865: 858: 853: 849: 844: 840: 835: 831: 826: 822: 817: 813: 808: 799: 790: 786: 773: 769: 764: 755: 750: 739: 730: 721: 716: 709: 704: 697: 692: 681: 676: 667: 662: 637: 633: 601: 568: 516:dwarf mongooses 495: 460: 454: 417: 384: 333:Equus burchelli 315:. For example, 302: 297: 287: 257: 241:Junco hymenalis 214:largemouth bass 182: 122:nutrient intake 73: 12: 11: 5: 1019: 1009: 1008: 993: 992: 983: 974: 965: 953: 941: 932: 923: 914: 902: 889: 880: 868: 856: 847: 838: 829: 820: 811: 797: 784: 767: 753: 737: 719: 707: 695: 679: 665: 634: 632: 629: 628: 627: 622: 617: 612: 607: 600: 597: 567: 564: 494: 491: 453: 450: 442:house sparrows 434:Rhea americana 416: 413: 391:cichlid fish ( 383: 380: 301: 298: 286: 285:Group foraging 283: 256: 253: 181: 178: 114:Grey squirrels 72: 69: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1018: 1007: 1004: 1003: 1001: 987: 978: 969: 960: 958: 948: 946: 936: 927: 918: 909: 907: 899: 893: 884: 875: 873: 863: 861: 851: 842: 833: 824: 815: 806: 804: 802: 794: 788: 781: 777: 776:Asio flammeus 771: 762: 760: 758: 748: 746: 744: 742: 734: 728: 726: 724: 714: 712: 702: 700: 690: 688: 686: 684: 674: 672: 670: 660: 658: 656: 654: 652: 650: 648: 646: 644: 642: 640: 635: 626: 623: 621: 618: 616: 615:Social animal 613: 611: 608: 606: 603: 602: 596: 594: 590: 586: 582: 576: 574: 563: 560: 556: 551: 546: 544: 540: 539:pied babblers 535: 534:on the prey. 533: 529: 525: 521: 517: 508: 504: 499: 490: 488: 484: 480: 479:Water skaters 476: 473: 469: 466: 459: 452:Alarm signals 449: 447: 443: 439: 435: 431: 430:greater rheas 427: 423: 412: 410: 406: 402: 398: 394: 390: 382:Disadvantages 379: 377: 372: 368: 360: 356: 352: 350: 346: 342: 338: 334: 330: 326: 325:cooperatively 322: 318: 314: 310: 309:Group hunting 306: 296: 292: 282: 279: 275: 271: 266: 262: 252: 250: 246: 242: 238: 233: 227: 225: 221: 217: 215: 211: 207: 203: 199: 195: 190: 187: 177: 175: 171: 167: 163: 159: 155: 151: 145: 143: 139: 138:Harris's hawk 134: 130: 125: 123: 119: 115: 108: 104: 103:grey squirrel 99: 95: 93: 89: 85: 83: 78: 77:energy intake 68: 65: 64:Alarm signals 61: 57: 53: 49: 45: 40: 38: 34: 30: 26: 22: 18: 986: 977: 968: 935: 926: 917: 897: 892: 883: 850: 841: 832: 823: 814: 792: 787: 779: 775: 770: 732: 592: 584: 577: 569: 547: 542: 536: 519: 512: 506: 482: 477: 470: 461: 458:Alarm signal 445: 433: 425: 418: 404: 392: 385: 364: 340: 332: 321:Panthera leo 320: 307: 303: 273: 270:Wood pigeons 258: 240: 228: 218: 197: 191: 183: 161: 146: 141: 126: 117: 112: 106: 86: 74: 55: 47: 41: 16: 15: 409:competition 170:water fleas 631:References 524:arthropods 411:for food. 389:Blue acara 337:wildebeest 300:Advantages 154:starvation 92:physiology 532:olfaction 522:) dig up 493:Sentinels 422:ostriches 395:) choose 345:positions 317:lionesses 224:moonlight 129:abundance 33:trade-off 17:Vigilance 1000:Category 599:See also 581:Cheetahs 566:Cheating 465:surprise 278:goshawks 261:solitary 202:plankton 186:feedback 52:meerkats 29:foraging 25:predator 555:jackals 503:meerkat 401:guppies 376:mobbing 367:dilutes 323:) hunt 166:density 528:vision 472:Flocks 428:) and 397:shoals 335:) and 313:ambush 293:, and 245:flocks 208:or on 174:juncos 150:hungry 133:detect 82:mating 50:) and 37:hunger 559:eagle 487:touch 349:panic 329:zebra 265:group 259:Both 232:Seeds 206:reeds 530:and 263:and 573:ESS 399:of 168:of 1002:: 956:^ 944:^ 905:^ 871:^ 859:^ 800:^ 756:^ 740:^ 722:^ 710:^ 698:^ 682:^ 668:^ 638:^ 557:, 501:A 101:A 39:. 591:( 583:( 541:( 518:( 505:( 481:( 444:( 432:( 424:( 403:( 339:( 331:( 319:( 272:( 239:( 196:( 160:( 140:( 116:( 105:( 54:( 46:(

Index

behavioural ecology
predator
foraging
trade-off
hunger
yellow-eyed juncos
meerkats
dilutes an individual's risk of predation
Alarm signals
energy intake
mating
Optimality models
physiology

grey squirrel
Grey squirrels
nutrient intake
abundance
detect
Harris's hawk
hungry
starvation
three-spined sticklebacks
density
water fleas
juncos
feedback
bluegill sunfish
plankton
reeds

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.