Knowledge

Vigilance (psychology)

Source 📝

123:, or SDT, in 1966 to characterize detection task performance sensitivity while accounting for both the observer's perceptual ability and willingness to respond. SDT assumes an active observer making perceptual judgments as conditions of uncertainty vary. A decision maker can vary their response bias, characterized by Beta, to allow more or less correct detections (hits), but at the respective cost of more or less false alarms. This is termed a criterion shift. The degree to which the observer tolerates false alarms to achieve a higher rate of detection is termed the bias. Bias represents a strategy to minimize the consequences of missed targets and false alarms. As an example, the lookout during a bank robbery must set a threshold for how "cop-like" an approaching individual or vehicle may be. Failing to detect the "cop" in a timely fashion may result in jail time, but a false alarm will result in a lost opportunity to steal money. In order to produce a bias-free measure, d' is calculated by measuring the distance between the means of the signal and non-signals (noise) and scaling by the standard deviation of the noise. Mathematically, this can be accomplished by subtracting the z-score of the hit rate from the z-score of the false alarm rate. Application of SDT to the study of vigilance indicates that in most, but not all cases, vigilance decrement is not the result of a reduction in sensitivity over time. In most cases a reduction of detections is accompanied by a commensurate reduction in false alarms, such that d' is relatively unchanged. 135:, based on task differences can significantly affect the degree of vigilance decrement. In 1977, Parasuraman and Davies investigated the effect of two task difference variables on d', and proposed the existence of a vigilance taxonomy based on discrimination type and event rate. Parasuraman and Davies employed discrimination tasks which were either successive or simultaneous, and presented both at high and low event rates. Successive discrimination tasks where critical information must be retained in working memory generate a greater mental workload than simultaneous comparison tasks. Their results indicate the type of discrimination and the rate at which discriminable events occur interact to affect sustained attention. Successive discrimination tasks indicate a greater degree of vigilance decrement than simultaneous discriminations, such as comparisons, but only when event rates are relatively high. For detection tasks, empirical evidence suggests that an event rate at or above 24 events per minute significantly reduces sensitivity. Further investigation has indicated that when the discrimination task is difficult, a decrement can occur when the mental workload is low, as with simultaneous comparisons, at both high and low event rates. 278:
task maintain that level of performance over time. For different tasks, however, individual performance differences are not consistent for any one individual may not correlate well from one task to another. An individual exhibiting no significant decrement while performing a counting monitoring task may exhibit a significant decrement during a clock test. Relative performance between subjects may also vary based on the nature of the task. For example, subjects whose task performance is well correlated for a successive task may exhibit a poor performance correlation for a simultaneous task. Conversely, subjects performing similar monitoring tasks, such as radar versus sonar target detection, can be expected to exhibit similar patterns of task performance.
321:
19 and 27, 40 and 55, and 70 and 80 years old. Deaton and Parasuraman reported in 1993 that beyond the age of 40 years, a trend towards lower detection rates and higher false alarm rates occurs in both cognitive tasks and sensory tasks, with higher and lower mental workloads respectively. Berardi, Parasuraman and Haxby reported no differences in 2001 in the overall levels of vigilance and the ability to sustain attention over time for when comparing middle aged (over 40) and younger subjects. Age dependent differences in cognitive tasks may differ with task type and workload, and some differences in detection and false alarms may be due to the reduction in the sensitivity of sensory organs.
333:. Habituation is the decrease in neural responsivity due to repeated stimulation. Under passive conditions, when no task is performed, participants exhibit attenuated N100 Event Related Potentials (ERP) that indicate neural habituation, and it was assumed that habituation was also responsible for the vigilance decrement. More recent ERP studies indicate that when performance declines during a vigilance task, N100 amplitude was not diminished. These results indicate that vigilance decrement is not the result of boredom or a reduction in neurological sensitivity. 17: 94:. Participants were tasked to report when they detected the larger jumps. Mackworth's results indicated a decline in signal detection over time, known as a vigilance decrement. The participants' event detection declined between 10 and 15 percent in the first 30 minutes and then continued to decline more gradually for the remaining 90 minutes. Mackworth's method became known as the "Clock Test" and this method has been employed in subsequent investigations. 299:
detection and false alarm rates to determine d', the measure of sensitivity. Participants dosed with amphetamine exhibited no increased sensitivity but did exhibit a highly significant reduction in vigilance decrement. In feedback trials, sensitivity increased while the performance decline was significantly reduced. In trials where both amphetamine and feedback were given, sensitivity was increased and there was no significant vigilance decrement.
165:
Research employing these techniques has linked increases in mental workload and allocation of attentional resources with increased activity in the prefrontal cortex. Studies employing PET, fMRI and TCD indicate a decline in activity in the prefrontal cortex correlates with vigilance decrement. Neuroimaging studies also indicate that the control of vigilance may reside in the right cerebral hemisphere in a variety of brain regions.
320:
Vigilance research conducted with subjects across a range of ages conflict regarding the ability to maintain alertness and sustained attention with age. In 1991, Parasuraman and Giambra reported a trend towards lower detection rates and higher false alarm rates with age when comparing groups between
277:
Large individual differences in monitoring task performance have been reported in a number of vigilance studies. For a given task, however, the vigilance decrement between subjects is generally consistent over time, such that individuals exhibiting relatively higher levels of performance for a given
307:
Training and practice significantly reduce the vigilance decrement, reduce the false alarm rate, and may improve sensitivity for many sustained attention tasks. Changes in strategy or bias may improve task performance. Improvements based on such a criterion shift would be expected to occur early in
106:
Under most conditions, vigilance decrement becomes significant within the first 15 minutes of attention, but a decline in detection performance can occur more quickly if the task demand conditions are high. This occurs in both experienced and novice task performers. Vigilance had traditionally been
89:
operators to miss rare irregular event detections near the end of their watch. Mackworth simulated rare irregular events on a radar display by having the test participants watch an unmarked clock face over a 2-hour period. A single clock hand moved in small equal increments around the clock face,
311:
Training improvements may also occur due to the reduced mental workload associated with task automaticity. In pilotage and airport security screening experiments, trained or expert subjects exhibit better detection of low salience targets, a reduction in false alarms, improved sensitivity, and a
151:
Initial Vigilance Taxonomy studies relied on assumptions regarding the mental workload associated with discrimination tasks, rather than a direct quantification of that workload. Successive discriminations, for example, were assumed to impose a greater workload than simultaneous discriminations.
102:
Vigilance decrement is defined as "deterioration in the ability to remain vigilant for critical signals with time, as indicated by a decline in the rate of the correct detection of signals". Vigilance decrement is most commonly associated with monitoring to detect a weak target signal. Detection
298:
could reduce or counteract the vigilance decrement. In 1965, Jane Mackworth conducted clock test experiments in which half of 56 participants were given a strong amphetamine and half were given a placebo. Mackworth also provided false feedback and feedback in separate trials. Mackworth analyzed
290:
Considerable research has been devoted to the reduction of the vigilance decrement. As noted above, the addition of non-target signals can improve task performance over time if the signals are similar to the target signals. Additionally, practice, performance feedback, amphetamines and rest are
281:
Levine et al. propose that individual differences in task performance may be influenced by task demands. For example, some tasks may require rapid comparisons or "perceptual speed", while others may require "flexibility of closure", such as detection of some predefined object within a cluttered
268:
are correlated with continuous extensive mental workloads, making these compounds good chemical indicators of stress levels. Subjects performing vigilance tasks exhibit elevated levels of epinephrine and norepinephrine, consistent with high stress levels and indicative of a significant mental
164:
sonography (TCD) have been employed to independently assess brain activation and mental workload during vigilance experiments. These neuroimaging techniques estimate brain activation by measuring the blood flow (fMRI and TCD) or glucose metabolism (PET) associated with specific brain regions.
142:
Other dimensions beyond event rate and discrimination task difficulty affect the performance of vigilance tasks and are factors in the Vigilance Taxonomy. These include but are not limited to: sensory modality, or combinations of sensory modalities; source complexity; signal duration; signal
138:
The effect of event rate on monitoring task performance can be affected by the addition of non-target salient objects at varying frequencies. Clock test research conducted in the late 1950s and 1960s indicates that an increase in event rate for rare irregular low salience signals reduced the
243:. Chemically blocking the release of noradrenaline induces drowsiness and lapses in attention associated with a vigilance decrement. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex exhibits a higher level of activation than other significantly active areas, indicating a key role in vigilance. 107:
associated with low cognitive demand and vigilance decrement with a decline in arousal pursuant to the low cognitive demand, but later studies indicated that vigilance is hard work, requiring the allocation of significant cognitive resources, and inducing significant levels of
103:
performance loss is less likely to occur in cases where the target signal exhibits a high saliency. For example, a radar operator would be unlikely to miss a rare target at the end of a watch if it were a large bright flashing signal, but might miss a small dim signal.
282:
scene. Linking task performance differences to task demands is consistent with the Vigilance Taxonomy proposed by Parasuraman and Davies described above, and also supports the hypothesis that vigilance requires mental work, rather than being a passive activity.
139:
vigilance decrement. When non-target "artificial" signals similar to target signals were introduced, the vigilance decrement was also reduced. When the "artificial" signal differed significantly from the target signal, no performance improvement was measured.
234:
and TCD data indicate that brain activation increases during vigilance tasks with greater activation in the right hemisphere. Lesion and split brain studies indicate better right-brain performance on vigilance tasks, indicating an important role for the
184:
associated with arousal play a critical role in the performance of vigilance tasks. Because the amygdala plays an important role in the recognition of emotional stimuli, it appears to be an important brain structure in the regulation of vigilance.
624:
Levine, J.M., Romashko, T., & Fleishman, E.A. (1973) Evaluation of an abilities classification system for integrating and generalizing human performance research findings: An application to vigilance tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology 58,
259:
Stressful activities involve continuous application of extensive cognitive resources. If the vigilance decrement were the result of less brain activity rather than more, vigilance tasks could not be expected to be stressful. High levels of
246:
The cingulate gyrus differs from other brain regions associated with vigilance in that it exhibits less activation during vigilance tasks. The role of the cingulate gyrus in vigilance is unclear, but its proximity and connections to the
52:
The study of vigilance has expanded since the 1940s mainly due to the increased interaction of people with machines for applications involving monitoring and detection of rare events and weak signals. Such applications include
408:
Parasuraman, R. (1986). Vigilance, Monitoring and Search In J.R. Boff, L. Kaufmann & J.P. Thomas (Eds.) Handbook of Human Perception and Performance, Vol.2, Cognitive Processes and Performance (pp 41–1 - 41-49). New York,
517:
Parasuraman, R. & Caggiano, G. (2005). Neural and genetic assays of mental workload. In D. McBride and D. Schmorrow (Ed.) Quantifying Human Information Processing (pp 123–155). Lanham, Maryland: Rowman and
48:
over prolonged periods of time. During this time, the person attempts to detect the appearance of a particular target stimulus. The individual watches for a signal stimulus that may occur at an unknown time.
569:
Shaw, T.H., Warm, J.S., Finomore, V., Tripp, L., Matthews, G., Weiler, E. & Parasuraman, R. (2009) Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance, Neuroscience Letters 461,
588:
Lundberg, U. & Frankenhaeuser, M. (1979) Pituitary-adrenal and sympathetic adrenal correlates of distress and effort (Report 548). Stockholm, Sweden: University of Stockholm, Department of Psychology
454:
Parasuraman, R. & Davies, D.R. (1977). A taxonomic analysis of vigilance. In R.R. Mackie, (ed.) Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates, (pp 559–574) New York: Plenum.
251:, which regulates interhemispheric activity, may be significant. Reduced activation in the cingulate gyrus may be a by-product of asymmetrical frontal lobe activation initiated in the corpus callosum. 81:. Mackworth authored "The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search" in 1948 and this paper is the seminal publication on vigilance. Mackworth's 1948 study investigated the tendency of 508:
Parasuraman, R. & Davies, D. (1977) A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In R. Mackie (ed.) Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates. Plenum Press.
308:
the training process. Experiments involving both audio and visual stimuli indicate the expected training performance improvement within the first five to ten hours of practice or less.
615:
Parasuraman, R. (1976) Consistency of Individual Differences of Human Vigilance Performance: An Abilities Classification Analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology Vol. 61 No. 4 486-492
778:
Rohrbaugh, J.W., Stapleton, J.M., Parasuraman, R., & Zubovic, E.A. (1987) Dose-related effects of ethanol on sustained attention and Event Related Potentials. Alcohol 4:293-300
670:
Wiliges, R.C. (1976) The vigilance increment: An ideal observer hypothesis. In T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen (Eds.) Monitoring Behavior and Supervisory Control. New York: Plenum
688:
Kerkhoff, G.A., van der Schaaf, T.W. & Korving, H.J. (1980) Auditory Signal Detection: Effects of long-term practice and time on task. Perception and Psychophysics 28, 79-81
481:
Neuchterliein, K.H., Parasuraman, R. & Jiang, Q. (1983). Visual sustained attention: Image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. Science, 220. 327-329.
560:
Moore, H., Sarter, M. & Bruno, J.P. (1993) Bidirectional modulation of stimulated cortical acetylcholine release by benzodiazepine receptor ligands. Brain Res. 596: 17-29
769:
Parasuraman, R. (1985) Sustained attention: a multifactorial approach. IN Attention and performance XI. M. Posner and O Marin, (Eds.) 493-511. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
223: 724:
Fisk, A.D. & Schneider, W. (1981) Control and automatic processing during tasks requiring sustained attention: A new approach to vigilance. Human Factors 23, 737-750.
143:
intensity; multiple signal sources; discrete versus continuous events; intermittent versus continuous attention requirement; observer skill level; and stimulation value.
181: 706:
Uenking, M. (2000) Pilot Biofeedback Training in the Cognitive Awareness Training Study. American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronomics, Proceedings 2000-4074, 1-8
751:
Berardi, A., Parasuraman, R. & Haxby, J. (2001) Overall vigilance and Sustained Attention Decrements in Healthy Aging. Experimental Aging Research 27, 19-39.
715:
McCarley, J.S., Kramer, A.F., Wickens, C.D., Vidoni, E.D., & Boot W.R. (2004) Visual Skills in Airport Security Screening. Psychological Science 15, 302-306
236: 196: 742:
Deaton, J. & Parasuraman, R. (1993) Sensory and Cognitive Vigilance: Effect of Age on Performance and Subjective Workload. Human Performance, 6(1), 71-97.
634:
Mackworth, Jane F. (1964) Performance Decrement in Vigilance, Threshold, and High-Speed Perceptual Motor Tasks. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 18(3). 209-223
329:
Early theories of vigilance explained the reduction of electrophysiological activity over time associated with the vigilance decrement as a result of neural
551:
Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. & See, J. (1998) Brain Systems of Vigilance in The Attentive Brain R. Parasuraman (Ed.) pp. 221–256. Cambridge MA: MIT Press
643:
Solandt, D.Y. & Partridge, D.M. (1946) Research on auditory problems presented by naval operations. Journal of the Canadian Medical Service3. 323-329
215: 661:
Mackworth, J.F. (1965) The effect of amphetamine on the detectability of signals in a vigilance task. Canadian Journal of Psychology 19 (2). 104-117
697:
Moray N., Fitter, M. Ostry, D., Favreau D., & Nagy, V. (1976) Attention to pure tones. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28, 271-283
427:
Helton et al. (2007). The abbreviated vigilance task and cerebral hemodynamics. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 549-552.
375:
Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human factors, 50(3), 433-441.
177:
is defined as a component of vigilance, though it is not, as one may believe, the sole source of the main effect of the vigilance decrement.
733:
Parasuraman, R. & Giambra, L. (1991) Skill Development in Vigilance: Effects of Event Rate and Age. Psychology and Aging, 6(2), 155-169.
679:
Mackworth, J.F. (1963) Effect of Reference Marks on the Detection of Signals on a Clock Face. Journal of Applied Psychology, 47(3). 196-201.
436:
Mackie, R.R. (1984). Research relevance and the information glut. Human Factors Review, (pp 1–11), Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
399:
Mackworth, N.H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6-21.
239:
in vigilance tasks. Activity in the LC noradrenergic system is associated with the alert waking state in animals through the release of
606:
Baker, C.H. (1963) Consistency of performance in two human vigilance tasks. Vigilance: A Symposium. New York: McGraw-Hill Performance
527:
Moruzzi, G. and Magoun, H.W. (1949) Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. EEG Clinical Neurophysiology I: 455-473
499:
Mackworth, Jane F. (1963) The Effect of Intermittent Signal Probability on Vigilance. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17(1). 82-89
157: 418:
Teichner, W.H. (1974). The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors 16, 339-353.
652:
Mackworth, N.H. (1950) Researches on the measurement of human performance. Med .Res .Council, Special Report., No. 268
791: 312:
significantly reduced vigilance decrement. In some cases the vigilance decrement was eliminated or not apparent.
207:
release, which is associated with cortical arousal. Blocking the release of acetylcholine in the forebrain with
214:
Several cortical brain regions are associated with attention and vigilance. These include the right frontal,
490:
Williams, P.S. (1986). Processing demands, training and the vigilance decrement. Human Factors, 28. 567-579.
153: 200: 445:
Frankmann, J.P. & Adams, J.A. (1962). Theories of Vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 59, 257-272.
357: 208: 120: 25: 463:
Green, D.M. & Swets, J.A. (1966) Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: Wiley
597:
Davies, D.R. & Tune, G.S. (1969) Human Vigilance Performance. New York: American Elsevier
161: 8: 347: 54: 812: 192: 269:
workload. Vigilance tasks may therefore be assumed to be stressful, hard mental work.
219: 16: 108: 74: 248: 227: 189: 91: 58: 291:
believed to moderate temporal performance decline without reducing sensitivity.
792:
https://web.archive.org/web/20100510232049/http://www.hfes.org/web/Default.aspx
265: 132: 90:
with the exception of occasional larger jumps. This device became known as the
806: 352: 240: 204: 295: 78: 203:(RAS). The basal forebrain cholinergic system is associated with cortical 330: 261: 126: 760:
Mackworth, J.F. (1969) Vigilance and Habituation. Baltimore MD: Penguin
294:
Beginning in the mid-1940s research was conducted to determine whether
173:
Reductions in arousal generally correspond to reductions in vigilance.
33: 472:
Broadbent, D.E. (1971) Decision and Stress. New York: Academic Press.
342: 285: 62: 45: 41: 146: 114: 174: 21: 188:
Subcortical brain regions associated with arousal include the
61:, automated navigation, military and border surveillance, and 152:
Beginning in the late 1990s, neuroimaging techniques such as
86: 82: 579:
Steriade, M. (1996) Awakening the brain. Nature 383: 24 - 25
231: 541:. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. p. 145. 272: 389:. Belmont: CA: Wadworth Cengage Learning. p. 142. 127:
Vigilance Taxonomy: discrimination type and event rate
797: 44:, is defined as the ability to maintain concentrated 302: 73:The systematic study of vigilance was initiated by 286:Reducing the vigilance decrement with amphetamines 804: 147:Measuring mental workload during vigilance tasks 115:Vigilance decrement and Signal Detection Theory 168: 536: 384: 15: 805: 324: 97: 68: 790:Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 273:Individual differences in performance 199:. Both regions are components of the 158:functional magnetic resonance imaging 796:American Psychological Association 13: 14: 824: 784: 303:Practice and sustained attention 772: 763: 754: 745: 736: 727: 718: 709: 700: 691: 682: 673: 664: 655: 646: 637: 628: 618: 609: 600: 591: 582: 573: 563: 554: 545: 530: 521: 511: 502: 493: 484: 475: 466: 457: 211:impairs vigilance performance. 195:, and the locus coeruleus (LC) 119:Green and Swets formulated the 448: 439: 430: 421: 412: 402: 393: 378: 369: 1: 363: 154:positron emission tomography 7: 336: 201:reticular activating system 10: 829: 537:Sternberg, Robert (2009). 385:Sternberg, Robert (2009). 224:superior temporal cortices 254: 182:subcortical brain regions 315: 169:Associated brain regions 40:, also termed sustained 358:Signal Detection Theory 230:. In the frontal lobe, 121:Signal Detection Theory 29: 24:"lollipop lady" with 19: 539:Cognitive Psychology 387:Cognitive Psychology 237:right frontal cortex 197:noradrenergic system 162:Transcranial Doppler 131:Mental workload, or 26:St. Paul's Cathedral 348:Attentional control 325:Lack of habituation 209:GABAergic compounds 98:Vigilance decrement 69:Origins of research 55:air traffic control 798:http://www.apa.org 193:cholinergic system 30: 28:in the background. 216:inferior parietal 57:, inspection and 820: 779: 776: 770: 767: 761: 758: 752: 749: 743: 740: 734: 731: 725: 722: 716: 713: 707: 704: 698: 695: 689: 686: 680: 677: 671: 668: 662: 659: 653: 650: 644: 641: 635: 632: 626: 622: 616: 613: 607: 604: 598: 595: 589: 586: 580: 577: 571: 567: 561: 558: 552: 549: 543: 542: 534: 528: 525: 519: 515: 509: 506: 500: 497: 491: 488: 482: 479: 473: 470: 464: 461: 455: 452: 446: 443: 437: 434: 428: 425: 419: 416: 410: 406: 400: 397: 391: 390: 382: 376: 373: 75:Norman Mackworth 828: 827: 823: 822: 821: 819: 818: 817: 803: 802: 787: 782: 777: 773: 768: 764: 759: 755: 750: 746: 741: 737: 732: 728: 723: 719: 714: 710: 705: 701: 696: 692: 687: 683: 678: 674: 669: 665: 660: 656: 651: 647: 642: 638: 633: 629: 623: 619: 614: 610: 605: 601: 596: 592: 587: 583: 578: 574: 568: 564: 559: 555: 550: 546: 535: 531: 526: 522: 516: 512: 507: 503: 498: 494: 489: 485: 480: 476: 471: 467: 462: 458: 453: 449: 444: 440: 435: 431: 426: 422: 417: 413: 407: 403: 398: 394: 383: 379: 374: 370: 366: 339: 327: 318: 305: 288: 275: 257: 249:corpus callosum 228:cingulate gyrus 190:basal forebrain 171: 149: 129: 117: 100: 92:Mackworth Clock 71: 59:quality control 12: 11: 5: 826: 816: 815: 801: 800: 794: 786: 785:External links 783: 781: 780: 771: 762: 753: 744: 735: 726: 717: 708: 699: 690: 681: 672: 663: 654: 645: 636: 627: 617: 608: 599: 590: 581: 572: 562: 553: 544: 529: 520: 510: 501: 492: 483: 474: 465: 456: 447: 438: 429: 420: 411: 401: 392: 377: 367: 365: 362: 361: 360: 355: 350: 345: 338: 335: 326: 323: 317: 314: 304: 301: 287: 284: 274: 271: 266:norepinephrine 256: 253: 170: 167: 148: 145: 133:cognitive load 128: 125: 116: 113: 99: 96: 70: 67: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 825: 814: 811: 810: 808: 799: 795: 793: 789: 788: 775: 766: 757: 748: 739: 730: 721: 712: 703: 694: 685: 676: 667: 658: 649: 640: 631: 621: 612: 603: 594: 585: 576: 566: 557: 548: 540: 533: 524: 514: 505: 496: 487: 478: 469: 460: 451: 442: 433: 424: 415: 405: 396: 388: 381: 372: 368: 359: 356: 354: 353:Human factors 351: 349: 346: 344: 341: 340: 334: 332: 322: 313: 309: 300: 297: 292: 283: 279: 270: 267: 263: 252: 250: 244: 242: 241:noradrenaline 238: 233: 229: 225: 221: 217: 212: 210: 206: 205:acetylcholine 202: 198: 194: 191: 186: 183: 178: 176: 166: 163: 159: 155: 144: 140: 136: 134: 124: 122: 112: 110: 104: 95: 93: 88: 84: 80: 76: 66: 64: 60: 56: 50: 47: 43: 42:concentration 39: 35: 27: 23: 18: 774: 765: 756: 747: 738: 729: 720: 711: 702: 693: 684: 675: 666: 657: 648: 639: 630: 620: 611: 602: 593: 584: 575: 565: 556: 547: 538: 532: 523: 518:Littlefield. 513: 504: 495: 486: 477: 468: 459: 450: 441: 432: 423: 414: 404: 395: 386: 380: 371: 328: 319: 310: 306: 296:amphetamines 293: 289: 280: 276: 258: 245: 213: 187: 179: 172: 150: 141: 137: 130: 118: 105: 101: 79:World War II 72: 63:lifeguarding 51: 37: 31: 331:habituation 262:epinephrine 160:(fMRI) and 364:References 220:prefrontal 34:psychology 32:In modern 813:Attention 343:Attention 180:As such, 46:attention 38:vigilance 807:Category 625:149-157. 337:See also 570:207-211 175:Arousal 156:(PET), 77:during 409:Wiley. 255:Stress 109:stress 22:London 316:Aging 87:sonar 83:radar 264:and 232:fMRI 226:and 85:and 809:: 222:, 218:, 111:. 65:. 36:, 20:A

Index


London
St. Paul's Cathedral
psychology
concentration
attention
air traffic control
quality control
lifeguarding
Norman Mackworth
World War II
radar
sonar
Mackworth Clock
stress
Signal Detection Theory
cognitive load
positron emission tomography
functional magnetic resonance imaging
Transcranial Doppler
Arousal
subcortical brain regions
basal forebrain
cholinergic system
noradrenergic system
reticular activating system
acetylcholine
GABAergic compounds
inferior parietal
prefrontal

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.