Knowledge

Polysynthetic language

Source 📝

1690:
concepts cluster more thickly, the words are more richly chambered, but there is a tendency, on the whole, to keep the range of concrete significance in the single word down to a moderate compass. A polysynthetic language, as its name implies, is more than ordinarily synthetic. The elaboration of the word is extreme. Concepts which we should never dream of treating in a subordinate fashion are symbolized by derivational affixes or “symbolic” changes in the radical element, while the more abstract notions, including the syntactic relations, may also be conveyed by the word. A polysynthetic language illustrates no principles that are not already exemplified in the more familiar synthetic languages. It is related to them very much as a synthetic language is related to our own analytic English. The three terms are purely quantitative—and relative, that is, a language may be “analytic” from one standpoint, “synthetic” from another. I believe the terms are more useful in defining certain drifts than as absolute counters. It is often illuminating to point out that a language has been becoming more and more analytic in the course of its history or that it shows signs of having crystallized from a simple analytic base into a highly synthetic form.
1546:
of a radical word as in the most European languages, but by interweaving together the most significant sounds or syllables of each simple word, so as to form a compound that will awaken in the mind at once all the ideas singly expressed by the words from which they are taken. 2. By an analogous combination of various parts of speech, particularly by means of the verb, so that its various forms and inflections will express not only the principal action, but the greatest possible number of the moral ideas and physical objects connected with it, and will combine itself to the greatest extent with those conceptions which are the subject of other parts of speech, and in other languages require to be expressed by separate and distinct words.... Their most remarkable external appearance is that of long polysyllabic words, which being compounded in the manner I have stated, express much at once.
1560:(1836), and it was long considered that all the indigenous languages of the Americas were of the same type. Humboldt considered language structure to be an expression of the psychological stage of evolution of a people, and since Native Americans were considered uncivilized, polysynthesis came to be seen as the lowest stage of grammatical evolution, characterized by a lack of rigorous rules and clear organization known in European languages. Duponceau himself had argued that the complex polysynthetic nature of American languages was a relic of a more civilized past, and that this suggested that the Indians of his time had degenerated from a previous advanced stage. Duponceau's colleague 1982: 603: 1678:
There is justification for the use of all of these terms, though not perhaps in quite the spirit in which they are commonly employed. In any case it is very difficult to assign all known languages to one or other of these groups, the more so as they are not mutually exclusive. A language may be both agglutinative and inflective, or inflective and polysynthetic, or even polysynthetic and isolating, as we shall see a little later on.
1673:, he argued that instead of using the morphological types as a strict classification scheme it made more sense to classify languages as relatively more or less synthetic or analytic, with the isolating and polysynthetic languages in each of the extremes of that spectrum. He also argued that languages were rarely purely of one morphological type, but used different morphological strategies in different parts of the grammar. 1777:, proposed an analysis of polysynthetic languages in which the morphemes that agree with the arguments of the verb are not just considered indexes of the arguments, but in fact constitute the primary expression of the arguments within the sentence. Because this theory posits that the pronominal agreement morphemes are the true syntactic arguments of the sentence, Jelinek's hypothesis was called the 679:, including derivational and inflectional morphemes. A language then is "synthetic" or "synthesizing" if it tends to have more than one morpheme per word, and a polysynthetic language is a language that has "many" morphemes per word. The concept was originally used only to describe those languages that can form long words that correspond to an entire sentence in English or other 1805:" program. He defines polysynthetic languages as languages that conform to the syntactic rule that he calls the "polysynthesis parameter", and that as a result show a special set of morphological and syntactic properties. The polysynthesis parameter states that all phrasal heads must be marked with either agreement morphemes of their direct argument or else 1944:
Because they possess a greater number of free morphemes, compositionally polysynthetic languages are much more prone than affixally polysynthetic ones to evolve into simpler languages with less complex words. On the other hand, they are generally easier to distinguish from non-polysynthetic languages
1700:
Sapir introduced a number of other distinctions according to which languages could be morphologically classified, and proposed combining them to form more complex classifications. He proposed classifying languages both by the degree of synthesis, classifying languages as either analytic, synthetic or
1521:
The manner in which words are compounded in that particular mode of speech, the great number and variety of ideas which it has the power of expressing in one single word; particularly by means of the verbs; all these stamp its character for abundance, strength, and comprehensiveness of expression, in
781:
as a means of syntactic cohesion. This means that many polysynthetic languages mark grammatical relations between verbs and their constituents by indexing the constituents on the verb with agreement morphemes, and the relation between noun phrases and their constituents by marking the head noun with
1657:
reacted to the prevailing view in Americanist linguistics which considered the languages of the Americas to belong to a single basic polysynthetic type, arguing instead that American indigenous languages were highly diverse and encompassed all known morphological types. He also built on the work of
1545:
construction of language.... It is that in which the greatest number of ideas are comprised in the fewest words. This is done principally in two ways. 1. By a mode of compounding locutions which is not confined to joining two words together, as in the Greek, or varying the inflection or termination
690:
between elements verbs and their arguments so that the verb is marked for agreement with the grammatical subject and object. In this way a single word can encode information about all the elements in a transitive clause. In Indo-European languages the verb is usually only marked for agreement with
1677:
Hence has arisen the still popular classification of languages into an “isolating” group, an “agglutinative” group, and an “inflective” group. Sometimes the languages of the American Indians are made to straggle along as an uncomfortable “polysynthetic” rear-guard to the agglutinative languages.
1689:
An analytic language is one that either does not combine concepts into single words at all (Chinese) or does so economically (English, French). In an analytic language the sentence is always of prime importance, the word is of minor interest. In a synthetic language (Latin, Arabic, Finnish) the
1627:
Their absence has not been demonstrated in any of which we have sufficient and authentic material on which to base a decision. The opinion of Du Ponceau and Humboldt, therefore, that these processes belong to the ground-plan of American languages, and are their leading characteristics, must be
1861:
from a basic root, which can lead to very complex word forms without non-lexical suffixes. These bound morphemes often relate to body parts, other essential items of the culture of the language's speakers or features of the landscape where the language is spoken. Deictics and other spatial and
1736:
and polysynthetic languages, where the former term was applied to languages with a very small number of morphemes of which all other lexical units are composed. No language has been shown to fit the description of an oligosynthetic language and the concept is not in general use in linguistics.
1564:
contradicted this theory, arguing rather that synthesis was a sign of a lower cultural level, and that while the Greek and Latin languages were somewhat synthetic, Native American languages were much more so – and consequently polysynthesis was the hallmark of the lowest level of intellectual
1865:
Affixally polysynthetic languages do not use noun incorporation or verb serialisation, since this violates the rule concerning the number of roots allowable per word. Many make a weak distinction between nouns and verbs, which allows using affixes to translate these parts of speech.
525:
At the same time, the question of whether to call a particular language polysynthetic is complicated by the fact that morpheme and word boundaries are not always clear cut, and languages may be highly synthetic in one area but less synthetic in other areas (e.g., verbs and nouns in
1592:, the first professor of anthropology in the US, followed Duponceau, Gallatin and Humboldt in seeing polysynthesis, which he distinguished from incorporation, as a defining feature of all the languages of the Americas. He defined polysynthesis in this way: 670:
is defined as a unit of meaning that can stand alone in a sentence, and which can be uttered in isolation. Words may be simple, consisting of a single unit of meaning, or they can be complex, formed by combining many small units of meaning, called
1832:
suggests that polysynthetic languages can be fundamentally divided into two typological categories, which differ in the way morphemes are organised to form words. She calls the two types for affixal and compositional polysynthesis respectively.
730:
Many polysynthetic languages combine these two strategies, and also have ways of inflecting verbs for concepts normally encoded by adverbs or adjectives in Indo-European languages. In this way highly complex words can be formed, for example the
1781:. If the hypothesis were correct, it would mean that free standing nouns in such languages did not constitute syntactical arguments, but simply adjoined specifiers or adjuncts. This in turn explained why many polysynthetic languages seem to be 2912:
Duponceau, Peter S. (1819). "Report of the corresponding secretary to the committee, of his progress in the investigation committed to him of the general character and forms of the languages of the American Indians: Read, 12th Jan. 1819.".
557:. This characterization was shown to be wrong, since many indigenous American languages are not polysynthetic, but it is a fact that polysynthetic languages are not evenly distributed throughout the world, but more frequent in the 1642:. Brinton, who had never done fieldwork with any indigenous group continued to defend Humboldt and Duponceau's view of the exceptional nature of American languages against the claim of Hewitt, who was half 1608:, etc., but also words, forms of words, and significant phonetic elements which have no separate existence apart from such compounds. This latter peculiarity marks it off altogether from the processes of 675:. In a general non-theoretical sense polysynthetic languages are those languages that have a high degree of morphological synthesis, and which tend to form long complex words containing long strings of 1938:
to create extremely long words. Bound affixes, though less important in compositionally polysynthetic languages than in affixally polysynthetic languages, tend to be equally abundant in both types.
1638:
In the 1890s the question of whether polysynthesis could be considered a general characteristic of Native American languages became a hotly contested issue as Brinton debated the question with
479:
have a low morpheme-to-word ratio, polysynthetic languages have a very high ratio. There is no generally agreed upon definition of polysynthesis. Generally polysynthetic languages have
1701:
polysynthetic, and by the technique used to achieve synthesis, classifying languages as agglutinative, fusional, or symbolic. Among the examples of polysynthetic languages he gave was
1941:
It is believed that all affixally polysynthetic languages evolved from compositionally polysynthetic ones via the conversion of morphemes that could stand on their own into affixes.
1778: 1572:
wrote in 1875. He considered polysynthesis to be a general characteristic of American languages, but he did qualify the statement by mentioning that certain languages such as
589:
theory of grammar. Other linguists question the basic utility of the concept for typology since it covers many separate morphological types that have little else in common.
1491:
That the American languages in general are rich in grammatical forms, and that in their complicated construction, the greatest order, method and regularity prevail
1809:
these arguments in that head. This definition of polysynthesis leaves out some languages that are commonly stated as examples of polysynthetic languages (such as
1785:, i.e. they have no strict rules for word order and seemingly violate many of the basic rules for syntactic structures posited within the generative framework. 325: 499:, also have it. Some authors apply the term polysynthetic to languages with high morpheme-to-word ratios, but others use it for languages that are highly 1829: 699:"I speak" where the -o ending marks agreement with the first person singular subject), but in many languages verbs also agree with the object (e.g. the 1821:. Baker's definition, probably because of its heavy dependence on generative theory, has not been accepted as a general definition of polysynthesis. 2916:
Transactions of the Historical & Literary Committee of the American Philosophical Society, held at Philadelphia, for promoting useful knowledge
3103: 2809:
Bickel, Balthasar; Nichols, Johanna (2011). "Chapter 22. Inflectional Synthesis of the Verb". In Dryer, Matthew S.; Haspelmath, Martin (eds.).
2987:(2003). "On the significance of Eloise Jelinek's Pronominal Argument Hypothesis". In Carnie, Andrew; Harley, Heidi; Willie, MaryAnn (eds.). 1646:
and had studied the Iroquoian languages, that languages such as the Iroquois had grammatical rules and verbs just like European languages.
3098:. North American Indians: Humanistic Perspectives. James S. Thayer, ed. University of Oklahoma Papers in Anthropology 24(2): 221–242. 318: 1758:
theoretical framework that operates with the assumption that all the world's languages share a set of basic syntactic principles.
1999: 1596:
Polysynthesis is a method of word-building, applicable either to nominals or verbals, which not only employs juxtaposition, with
620: 246: 241: 236: 224: 219: 214: 81: 3138: 3095: 3066: 2848: 2732: 2707: 1876:, with a fixed number of slots for different elements, which are fixed in their position and order relative to each other; or 142: 3008:Über die Verschiedenheit des menschichen Sprachbaues und ihren Einfluß auf die geistige Entwicklung des Menschengeschlechts 554: 2884:
American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America (Oxford Studies in Anthropological Linguistics, 4)
751:
with the meanings, reindeer-hunt-future-say-negation-again-third.person.singular.indicative, and except for the morpheme
311: 132: 1483:(a.k.a. Pierre Étienne Du Ponceau) in 1819 as a term to describe the structural characteristics of American languages: 95: 3225: 3173: 3113: 3085: 2966: 2933: 2895: 2782: 2615: 2021: 642: 553:, who considered polysynthesis, as characterized by sentence words and noun incorporation, a defining feature of all 1666:
dismissed morphological typology, stating specifically that the term polysynthetic had never been clearly defined.
1509:
That these forms appear to differ essentially from those of the ancient and modern languages of the old hemisphere.
573:; however, there are also examples in other areas. The concept became part of linguistic typology with the work of 107: 2245: 1152: 2003: 743:
which means "He had not yet said again that he was going to hunt reindeer." The word consists of the morphemes
624: 2072: 2040: 1782: 1750:
The sentence structure of polysynthetic languages has been taken as a challenge for linguists working within
527: 263: 3262: 1639: 362:(word parts that have independent meaning but may or may not be able to stand alone). They are very highly 137: 1862:
temporal relations are also very common among these bound morphemes in affixally polysynthetic languages.
2249: 1806: 803: 763: 122: 2201: 2094: 1891: 783: 231: 209: 166: 100: 2949:
Fortescue, Michael. (1994). Morphology, polysynthetic. In R. E. Asher & J. M. Y. Simpson (Eds.),
1802: 586: 273: 90: 2169: 1522:
such a manner, that those accidents must be considered as included in the general descriptive term
550: 2887: 2867: 2774: 1992: 1882:, with forms not restricted in complexity and length. The components are fixed in their relative 1597: 1589: 1480: 687: 680: 613: 253: 171: 70: 3126: 2836: 2261: 2253: 2159: 1755: 1577: 1569: 807: 762:
Another way to achieve a high degree of synthesis is when languages can form compound words by
511: 484: 480: 339: 60: 29: 1886:
and are thus ordered according to the intended meaning. Usually in this case a few components
2766: 2119: 2062: 1845:
to express concepts that in less synthetic languages are expressed by separate words such as
1813:), but can be seen as the reason for certain common structural properties in others, such as 1794: 778: 578: 500: 299: 181: 176: 161: 2641:
See Mattissen: "On the Ontology and Diachronisis of Polysynthesis"; in Wunderlich (editor):
2164: 2084: 1907: 1774: 1557: 1331: 152: 147: 127: 1841:
Affixally polysynthetic languages, as the name suggests, are those that use only non-root-
8: 2212: 2154: 2099: 1965: 1911: 1601: 1234: 22: 3214: 3041: 2944:
A comparative manual of affixes for the Inuit dialects of Greenland, Canada, and Alaska
2672: 2206: 2196: 2186: 2124: 2109: 1935: 1919: 1810: 1770: 1729: 1659: 1050:
is also considered to be polysynthetic, incorporating and agglutinating. The following
504: 492: 476: 355: 351: 50: 43: 3231: 3221: 3204: 3169: 3134: 3109: 3081: 3062: 3045: 2972: 2962: 2946:. Meddelelser om Grønland, Man & society (No. 4). Copenhagen: Nyt Nordisk Forlag. 2929: 2891: 2844: 2788: 2778: 2728: 2721:
Fortescue, Michael; Fortescue, Michael D.; Mithun, Marianne; Evans, Nicholas (2017).
2611: 2282: 2191: 2104: 2089: 1915: 1903: 1718: 1059: 1055: 515: 496: 117: 55: 38: 366:
languages. Polysynthetic languages typically have long "sentence-words" such as the
3267: 3193:
Language typology and syntactic description: Grammatical categories and the lexicon
3147:
Sapir, Edward. (1911). Problem of noun incorporation in American Indian languages.
3033: 2995:
Hewitt, John N. B. (1893). Polysynthesis in the languages of the American Indians.
2751:
Pentland, David H. (2006). "Algonquian and Ritwan Languages", in Keith Brown, ed.,
2664: 2230: 2217: 2114: 1961: 1957: 1818: 1722: 1714: 1643: 1063: 811: 799: 700: 112: 2984: 2722: 2149: 2137: 2067: 1814: 1710: 1561: 1237: 531: 488: 268: 186: 3078:
Dependent-head synthesis in Nivkh: a contribution to a typology of polysynthesis
3017:(pp. 2–3). Fairbanks: Alaska Native Languages Center, University of Alaska. 1761: 1487:
Three principal results have forcibly struck my mind... They are the following:
683:, and the word is still most frequently used to refer to such "sentence words". 3021: 2802:
Science Encounters the Indian, 1820-1880: The Early Years of American Ethnology
2238: 2234: 2174: 2051: 1930:
In compositionally polysynthetic languages, there usually can be more than one
1842: 1766: 1733: 1702: 1573: 732: 367: 3256: 2976: 2879: 2257: 2133: 2129: 2057: 1953: 1949: 1931: 1706: 1609: 1155: 888: 787: 535: 258: 2792: 1797:
proposed a definition of polysynthesis as a syntactic macroparameter within
891:
of Japan, another polysynthetic, incorporating, and agglutinating language:
518:
depending on whether they encode one or multiple grammatical categories per
3235: 2045: 1869:
Affixally polysynthetic languages may have a word structure that is either
1798: 1751: 1654: 582: 574: 766:
of nouns, so that entire words can be incorporated into the verb word, as
581:
has suggested formally defining polysynthesis as a macro-parameter within
3163: 1824: 1613: 3201:
Grammatika čukotskogo jazyka: Fonetika i morfologija imennyx častej reči
2872:
Polysynthesis and incorporation as characteristics of American languages
1474: 3122:
de Reuse, Willem J. Central Siberian Yupik as a polysynthetic language.
3037: 2006: in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. 777:
Another common feature of polysynthetic languages is a tendency to use
627: in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. 570: 539: 363: 294: 200: 191: 2676: 1948:
Examples of compositionally polysynthetic languages include Classical
1041:'I keep swaying my heart afar and toward myself over various rumors'.) 719:
prefix marks agreement with the first person singular subject and the
1899: 1846: 1503: 1499: 562: 2989:
Formal Approaches to Function in Grammar: In honor of Eloise Jelinek
1981: 1455:
aliikku- sersu- i- llammas- sua- a- nerar- ta- ssa- galuar- paal- li
602: 577:, who used it as one of his basic typological categories. Recently, 2668: 676: 672: 558: 359: 1705:
which he considered to use the agglutinative-isolating technique,
1725:
he considered "mildly polysynthetic" and agglutinative-fusional.
1605: 1047: 759:"reindeer", none of the other morphemes can appear in isolation. 566: 472:"reindeer", none of the other morphemes can appear in isolation. 285: 2625: 2623: 1850: 462:"He had not yet said again that he was going to hunt reindeer." 2957:
Fortescue, Michael; Mithun, Marianne; Evans, Nicholas (2017).
2810: 1728:
Following Sapir's understanding of Polysynthesis, his student
1556:
The term was made popular in a posthumously published work by
814:
unlike the majority of incorporating polysynthetic languages:
727:
prefix marks agreement with a second person singular object).
686:
Often polysynthesis is achieved when languages have extensive
3129:(2006). "Canada: Language Situation". In Brown, Keith (ed.). 2720: 1762:
Non-configurationality and the pronominal argument hypothesis
1465:'However, they will say that he is a great entertainer, but…' 721: 713: 705: 543: 519: 2620: 786:
that may be considered to be polysynthetic because they use
1858: 1854: 1051: 667: 3220:. Cambridge Language Surveys. Cambridge University Press. 3024:(1984). "Empty categories, case, and configurationality". 2448: 1788: 1032:
various rumors {} 1S APL REFL APL far REFL heart sway ITER
2484: 1323:
again- PAST- 3S.F>2S.M- open.door- reversive- for.PERF
1070: 2991:. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. pp. 11–43. 2760:
Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing
2610:
Andronis, Mary; Chicago Linguistic Society #38; p. 386.
2592: 2580: 2568: 2556: 2472: 2318: 2316: 2314: 655:
The word "polysynthesis" is composed of the Greek roots
2839:(2006). "Australian Languages". In Brown, Keith (ed.). 2695: 2508: 2436: 2412: 1229:"They would carry the dead wallaby on their shoulders." 753: 745: 737: 467: 380: 371: 3158:
Osborne, C.R., 1974. The Tiwi language. Canberra: AIAS
2400: 2364: 1898:
Examples of affixally polysynthetic languages include
1825:
Johanna Mattissen's affixal and compositional subtypes
16:
Highly inflected language with many morphemes per word
2683: 2460: 2424: 2328: 2311: 1934:
per word, which gives rise to noun incorporation and
1649: 1475:
Peter Stephen Du Ponceau on Native American languages
1029:
usa- opuspe {} a- e- yay- ko- tuyma- si- ram- suy- pa
3105:
Describing morphosyntax: a guide for field linguists
2956: 2908:(2nd ed.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 2655:
Spencer, Andrew (1995). "Incorporation in Chukchi".
2352: 1853:. They also use these bound morphemes to make other 1462:
one.good.at COP say.that REFL FUT sure.but 3P/3S but
3055:
Variations on polysynthesis: the Eskaleut languages
2961:. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. 2520: 2388: 2376: 1334:, an exclusively suffixing polysynthetic language: 3213: 2773:. Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax. New York: 3010:. Berlin: Königliche Akadamie der Wissenschaften. 2544: 2496: 1628:regarded as still uncontroverted in any instance. 1338: 790:to achieve similar effects, and very long words. 3254: 3195:(Vol. 3). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 3165:Language: An introduction to the study of speech 3108:. Cambridge University Press. pp. 238–241. 2532: 534:). Many polysynthetic languages display complex 2755:(2nd ed.), pp. 161–6. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 1498:, appear to exist in all those languages, from 1282: 1226:3P 3S.F dead.wallaby carry.on.shoulders PST.HAB 1091: 1441: 1340:Aliikkusersuillammassuaanerartassagaluarpaalli 1274: 1183: 1081: 949: 867: 829: 487:languages that are not polysynthetic, such as 446: 3053:Mahieu, Marc-Antoine; Tersis, Nicole (2009). 3015:A grammatical sketch of Siberian Yupik Eskimo 2808: 1147:"I shall make somebody give something to you" 693: 459:reindeer -hunt -FUT -say -NEG -again -3SG.IND 319: 3052: 2951:The Encyclopedia of language and linguistics 2924:Evans, Nicholas; Sasse, Hans-Jürgen (2002). 2648: 1568:This view was still prevalent when linguist 1537:I have explained elsewhere what I mean by a 1437: 1244: 1173: 818: 456:tuntu -ssur -qatar -ni -ksaite -ngqiggte -uq 2906:Language universals and linguistic typology 2706:sfn error: no target: CITEREFPentland2006 ( 1713:both of which he considered agglutinative, 1580:had been claimed to be basically analytic. 1494:That these complicated forms, which I call 1479:The term "polysynthesis" was first used by 1038: 3131:Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics 2923: 2857: 2841:Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics 2490: 2418: 2334: 1740: 1162: 326: 312: 3182: 3168:. New York: Harcourt, Brace and company. 3075: 2911: 2753:Encyclopedia of Languages and Linguistics 2689: 2629: 2598: 2586: 2574: 2562: 2430: 2370: 2022:Learn how and when to remove this message 1469: 1158:is also considered highly polysynthetic: 979: 959: 643:Learn how and when to remove this message 427: 3187:. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2878: 2701: 2322: 1945:than affixally polysynthetic languages. 1745: 1286: 1023: 3242: 3125: 3020: 2866: 2654: 2526: 2478: 2466: 2454: 2406: 1789:Mark C. Baker's polysynthesis parameter 1278: 1217: 1187: 450: 3255: 3247:. New York: D. Appleton & Company. 3211: 3198: 3026:Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2812:The World Atlas of Language Structures 2799: 2442: 2382: 2358: 1459: 1373: 747:tuntu-ssur-qatar-ni-ksaite-ngqiggte-uq 3161: 3133:(Second ed.). Elsevier Science. 3101: 2843:(Second ed.). Elsevier Science. 2835: 2828:Handbook of American Indian languages 2765: 2643:Advances in the theory of the lexicon 2550: 2514: 2502: 2394: 2346: 1144:1S 2S someone something give CAUS FUT 871: 2983: 2959:The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis 2815:. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library 2724:The Oxford Handbook of Polysynthesis 2538: 2004:adding citations to reliable sources 1975: 1890:actually fixed, such as the root in 1419: 1223:Pi- ti- wuliyondji- rrurlimpirr- ani 1135: 782:agreement morphemes. There are some 770:is incorporated in the English verb 625:adding citations to reliable sources 596: 555:indigenous languages of the Americas 408: 3057:. Typological studies in language, 1326:"she opened the door for him again" 13: 2919:. Vol. 1. pp. xvii–xlvi. 2281:In this example, the subject "he" 1721:which he considered fusional. The 1650:Edward Sapir's morphological types 1263: 1213: 14: 3279: 3243:Whitney, William Dwight (1908) . 3216:The Mesoamerican Indian Languages 2874:. American Philosophical Society. 2862:. Motilal Banarsidass Publishers. 1669:In Sapir's 1921 book also titled 1409: 1141:ni- mits- teː- tla- maki- ltiː- s 1125: 1054:shows how the verb is marked for 1035:'I wonder about various rumors.' 997: 969: 2180: 2078: 1980: 1925: 1583: 914:Usaopuspe aeyaykotuymasiramsuypa 601: 3245:The life and growth of language 3006:von Humboldt, Wilhelm. (1836). 2804:. University of Oklahoma Press. 2714: 2635: 2604: 1991:needs additional citations for 1971: 1732:proposed a distinction between 1314: 880:1S.SUBJ great head hurt 1S.PRES 833: 612:needs additional citations for 510:Polysynthetic languages can be 503:, or those that frequently use 2340: 2298: 2275: 2143: 1779:pronominal argument hypothesis 1391: 739:tuntussuqatarniksaitengqiggtuq 382:tuntussuqatarniksaitengqiggtuq 373:tuntussuqatarniksaitengqiggtuq 1: 3183:Shibatani, Masayoshi (1990). 3102:Payne, Thomas Edward (1997). 3080:. John Benjamins Publishers. 2858:Bloomfield, Leonard (1995) . 2292: 2073:Northwest Caucasian languages 2041:Chukotko-Kamchatkan languages 1320:sa- honwa- nhoton- kw- a- hse 528:Southern Athabaskan languages 3013:Jacobson, Steven A. (1977). 2942:Fortescue, Michael. (1983). 1513:(Duponceau 1819: xxii–xxiii) 1164:Pitiwuliyondjirrurlimpirrani 906: 898: 663:meaning "placing together". 7: 3191:Shopen, Timothy. (1985). 3096:The genius of polysynthesis 3076:Mattissen, Johanna (2003). 2928:. Berlin: Akademie Verlag. 2771:The Polysynthesis Parameter 2727:. Oxford University Press. 883:'I have a fierce headache.' 793: 784:dependent-marking languages 354:, i.e., languages in which 143:Ditransitive/Monotransitive 10: 3284: 3203:. Vol. 1. Leningrad: 2800:Bieder, Robert E. (1986). 2744: 2223: 2034: 1836: 1551:(Duponceau 1819: xxx–xxxi) 819: 691:the subject (e.g. Spanish 592: 3212:Suárez, Jorge A. (1983). 2926:Problems of polysynthesis 2904:Comrie, Bernard. (1989). 2306:Dictionary of Linguistics 2304:Mario Pei, Frank Gaynor, 1803:principles and parameters 877:t- meyŋ- levt- pəγt- rkən 810:of Russia which also has 587:principles and parameters 549:The term was invented by 3094:Mithun, Marianne. 1983. 2268: 2170:Tequistlatecan languages 1458:entertainment provide - 1330:An example from Western 551:Peter Stephen Du Ponceau 466:Except for the morpheme 3199:Skorik, P. Ja. (1961). 3149:American Anthropologist 2997:American Anthropologist 2888:Oxford University Press 2775:Oxford University Press 1741:Contemporary approaches 1590:Daniel Garrison Brinton 1481:Peter Stephen DuPonceau 1309: 1301: 1293: 1270: 1259: 1251: 1245: 1209: 1201: 1193: 1179: 1169: 1163: 1131: 1121: 1113: 1105: 1097: 1087: 1077: 1071: 1066:, and indirect object: 1019: 1011: 1003: 993: 985: 975: 965: 955: 945: 937: 929: 921: 903: 895: 863: 855: 847: 839: 825: 754: 746: 738: 711:"I love you" where the 681:Indo-European languages 468: 441: 433: 422: 414: 403: 395: 387: 381: 372: 344:polysynthetic languages 3185:The languages of Japan 3162:Sapir, Edward (1921). 2419:Evans & Sasse 2002 2335:Evans & Sasse 2002 2202:Tupi–Guaraní languages 2160:Mixe-Zoquean languages 2095:Eskimo–Aleut languages 1892:Eskimo–Aleut languages 1698: 1686: 1636: 1624: 1578:Tupi-Guarani languages 1570:William Dwight Whitney 1554: 1534: 1531:(Duponceau 1819:xxvii) 1518: 1470:History of the concept 1447: 1433: 1425: 1415: 1405: 1397: 1387: 1379: 1369: 1361: 1353: 1345: 1339: 911:ウサオプㇲペ アエヤィコツ゚ィマシラㇺスィパ 907:aeyaykotuymasiramsuypa 808:agglutinating language 722: 714: 706: 694: 481:polypersonal agreement 348:holophrastic languages 2826:Boas, Franz. (1911). 2758:Baker, Mark. (1988). 2120:Uto-Aztecan languages 1769:, having worked with 1746:Generative approaches 1687: 1675: 1662:who in his 1914 work 1625: 1594: 1535: 1519: 1485: 1046:The Mexican language 358:are composed of many 133:Nominative–absolutive 96:Nominative–accusative 2246:Australian languages 2085:Algonquian languages 2000:improve this article 1908:Athabaskan languages 1775:Athabascan languages 1621:Brinton, 1885:14,15) 1558:Wilhelm von Humboldt 1072:Nimitztētlamaquiltīz 820:Təmeyŋəlevtpəγtərkən 621:improve this article 3263:Linguistic typology 3178:. ASIN: B000NGWX8I. 2632:, pp. 281–290. 2457:, pp. 258–262. 2213:Amazonian languages 2155:Totonacan languages 2100:Iroquoian languages 2063:Jiarongic languages 1966:Amazonian languages 1912:Chimakuan languages 1783:non-configurational 1246:Sahonwanhotónkwahse 1153:Australian language 802:, a polysynthetic, 659:meaning "many" and 477:isolating languages 352:synthetic languages 340:linguistic typology 247:Object–verb–subject 242:Object–subject–verb 237:Subject–object–verb 225:Verb–object–subject 220:Verb–subject–object 215:Subject–verb–object 108:Ergative–absolutive 23:Linguistic typology 3061:. John Benjamins. 3038:10.1007/BF00233713 2517:, pp. 150–51. 2207:Arawakan languages 2197:Quechuan languages 2187:Chibchan languages 2165:Purépecha language 2125:Wakashan languages 2110:Salishan languages 1936:verb serialisation 1920:Wakashan languages 1730:Benjamin Lee Whorf 1660:Leonard Bloomfield 1235:Iroquoian language 1205:carry.on.shoulders 505:noun incorporation 3140:978-0-08-044854-1 3068:978-90-272-0667-1 2850:978-0-08-044854-1 2734:978-0-19-968320-8 2481:, pp. 14–15. 2445:, pp. 29–34. 2250:Macro-Gunwinyguan 2192:Aymaran languages 2105:Na-Dene languages 2090:Caddoan languages 2032: 2031: 2024: 1830:Johanna Mattissen 1042: 812:grammatical cases 666:In linguistics a 653: 652: 645: 542:systems in their 336: 335: 278:Place–manner–time 274:Time–manner–place 167:Dependent-marking 118:Symmetrical voice 101:Marked nominative 3275: 3248: 3239: 3219: 3208: 3188: 3179: 3144: 3119: 3091: 3072: 3049: 2992: 2985:Hale, Kenneth L. 2980: 2939: 2920: 2901: 2875: 2863: 2854: 2823: 2821: 2820: 2805: 2796: 2739: 2738: 2718: 2712: 2711: 2699: 2693: 2687: 2681: 2680: 2652: 2646: 2639: 2633: 2627: 2618: 2608: 2602: 2596: 2590: 2584: 2578: 2572: 2566: 2560: 2554: 2548: 2542: 2536: 2530: 2524: 2518: 2512: 2506: 2500: 2494: 2488: 2482: 2476: 2470: 2464: 2458: 2452: 2446: 2440: 2434: 2428: 2422: 2416: 2410: 2404: 2398: 2392: 2386: 2380: 2374: 2368: 2362: 2356: 2350: 2344: 2338: 2332: 2326: 2320: 2309: 2302: 2286: 2283:has been dropped 2279: 2231:Papuan languages 2115:Siouan languages 2027: 2020: 2016: 2013: 2007: 1984: 1976: 1723:Siouan languages 1696: 1684: 1634: 1622: 1588:The ethnologist 1552: 1532: 1514: 1461: 1443: 1439: 1421: 1411: 1393: 1375: 1342: 1316: 1288: 1284: 1280: 1276: 1265: 1248: 1219: 1215: 1189: 1185: 1175: 1166: 1137: 1127: 1093: 1083: 1074: 1040: 1036: 1025: 999: 981: 971: 961: 951: 873: 869: 835: 831: 822: 821: 798:An example from 757: 749: 741: 725: 717: 709: 697: 648: 641: 637: 634: 628: 605: 597: 483:, although some 471: 452: 448: 429: 410: 384: 375: 328: 321: 314: 19: 18: 3283: 3282: 3278: 3277: 3276: 3274: 3273: 3272: 3253: 3252: 3251: 3228: 3176: 3141: 3116: 3088: 3069: 3022:Jelinek, Eloise 2969: 2936: 2898: 2851: 2818: 2816: 2785: 2747: 2742: 2735: 2719: 2715: 2705: 2700: 2696: 2688: 2684: 2653: 2649: 2640: 2636: 2628: 2621: 2609: 2605: 2597: 2593: 2585: 2581: 2573: 2569: 2561: 2557: 2549: 2545: 2537: 2533: 2525: 2521: 2513: 2509: 2501: 2497: 2491:Bloomfield 1995 2489: 2485: 2477: 2473: 2465: 2461: 2453: 2449: 2441: 2437: 2429: 2425: 2417: 2413: 2405: 2401: 2393: 2389: 2381: 2377: 2369: 2365: 2357: 2353: 2345: 2341: 2333: 2329: 2321: 2312: 2303: 2299: 2295: 2290: 2289: 2280: 2276: 2271: 2226: 2183: 2150:Mayan languages 2146: 2138:Hokan languages 2081: 2068:Munda languages 2037: 2028: 2017: 2011: 2008: 1997: 1985: 1974: 1928: 1843:bound morphemes 1839: 1827: 1791: 1764: 1748: 1743: 1697: 1694: 1685: 1682: 1652: 1635: 1633:Brinton 1885:41 1632: 1623: 1620: 1586: 1562:Albert Gallatin 1553: 1550: 1533: 1530: 1515: 1512: 1477: 1472: 1467: 1453: 1445: 1431: 1423: 1413: 1403: 1395: 1385: 1377: 1367: 1359: 1351: 1343: 1328: 1318: 1307: 1299: 1291: 1268: 1257: 1249: 1231: 1221: 1207: 1199: 1191: 1177: 1167: 1149: 1139: 1129: 1119: 1111: 1103: 1095: 1085: 1075: 1044: 1027: 1017: 1009: 1001: 991: 983: 973: 963: 953: 943: 935: 927: 918: 909: 904:アエヤィコツ゚ィマシラㇺスィパ 901: 887:From Classical 885: 875: 861: 853: 845: 837: 823: 796: 649: 638: 632: 629: 618: 606: 595: 532:Inuit languages 464: 454: 439: 431: 420: 412: 401: 393: 385: 332: 269:Free word order 187:Syntactic pivot 82:Morphosyntactic 17: 12: 11: 5: 3281: 3271: 3270: 3265: 3250: 3249: 3240: 3226: 3209: 3196: 3189: 3180: 3174: 3159: 3156: 3145: 3139: 3127:Rowicka, G. J. 3123: 3120: 3114: 3099: 3092: 3086: 3073: 3067: 3050: 3018: 3011: 3004: 2993: 2981: 2967: 2954: 2947: 2940: 2934: 2921: 2909: 2902: 2896: 2880:Campbell, Lyle 2876: 2868:Brinton, D. G. 2864: 2855: 2849: 2833: 2831: 2824: 2806: 2797: 2783: 2767:Baker, Mark C. 2763: 2756: 2748: 2746: 2743: 2741: 2740: 2733: 2713: 2704:, p. 163. 2694: 2690:Mattissen 2003 2682: 2669:10.2307/416217 2663:(3): 439–489. 2647: 2634: 2630:Mattissen 2003 2619: 2603: 2601:, p. 287. 2599:Mattissen 2003 2591: 2589:, p. 286. 2587:Mattissen 2003 2579: 2577:, p. 315. 2575:Mattissen 2003 2567: 2565:, p. 281. 2563:Mattissen 2003 2555: 2543: 2531: 2519: 2507: 2495: 2493:, p. 208. 2483: 2471: 2459: 2447: 2435: 2431:Duponceau 1819 2423: 2411: 2409:, p. 194. 2399: 2397:, p. 591. 2387: 2375: 2371:Shibatani 1990 2363: 2361:, p. 102. 2351: 2339: 2327: 2310: 2296: 2294: 2291: 2288: 2287: 2273: 2272: 2270: 2267: 2266: 2265: 2258:classical Tiwi 2242: 2225: 2222: 2221: 2220: 2215: 2209: 2204: 2199: 2194: 2189: 2182: 2179: 2178: 2177: 2175:Huave language 2172: 2167: 2162: 2157: 2152: 2145: 2142: 2141: 2140: 2127: 2122: 2117: 2112: 2107: 2102: 2097: 2092: 2087: 2080: 2077: 2076: 2075: 2070: 2065: 2060: 2055: 2049: 2043: 2036: 2033: 2030: 2029: 1988: 1986: 1979: 1973: 1970: 1927: 1924: 1896: 1895: 1877: 1838: 1835: 1826: 1823: 1790: 1787: 1767:Eloise Jelinek 1763: 1760: 1747: 1744: 1742: 1739: 1734:oligosynthetic 1692: 1680: 1651: 1648: 1630: 1618: 1585: 1582: 1548: 1528: 1517: 1516: 1510: 1507: 1492: 1476: 1473: 1471: 1468: 1446: 1432: 1424: 1414: 1404: 1396: 1386: 1378: 1368: 1360: 1352: 1344: 1337: 1336: 1308: 1300: 1292: 1269: 1258: 1250: 1243: 1242: 1208: 1200: 1192: 1178: 1168: 1161: 1160: 1130: 1120: 1112: 1104: 1096: 1086: 1076: 1069: 1068: 1018: 1010: 1002: 992: 984: 974: 964: 954: 944: 936: 928: 920: 919: 902: 894: 893: 862: 854: 846: 838: 824: 817: 816: 795: 792: 651: 650: 609: 607: 600: 594: 591: 440: 432: 421: 413: 402: 394: 386: 379: 378: 334: 333: 331: 330: 323: 316: 308: 305: 304: 303: 302: 297: 289: 288: 282: 281: 280: 279: 276: 271: 266: 261: 256: 251: 250: 249: 244: 239: 229: 228: 227: 222: 217: 204: 203: 197: 196: 195: 194: 189: 184: 179: 174: 172:Double-marking 169: 164: 159: 158: 157: 156: 155: 150: 140: 138:Direct-inverse 135: 130: 125: 123:Active–stative 120: 115: 113:Split ergative 110: 105: 104: 103: 85: 84: 78: 77: 76: 75: 74: 73: 71:Oligosynthetic 68: 63: 58: 48: 47: 46: 33: 32: 26: 25: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 3280: 3269: 3266: 3264: 3261: 3260: 3258: 3246: 3241: 3237: 3233: 3229: 3227:0-521-22834-4 3223: 3218: 3217: 3210: 3206: 3202: 3197: 3194: 3190: 3186: 3181: 3177: 3175:0-246-11074-0 3171: 3167: 3166: 3160: 3157: 3154: 3150: 3146: 3142: 3136: 3132: 3128: 3124: 3121: 3117: 3115:9780521588058 3111: 3107: 3106: 3100: 3097: 3093: 3089: 3087:1-58811-476-7 3083: 3079: 3074: 3070: 3064: 3060: 3056: 3051: 3047: 3043: 3039: 3035: 3031: 3027: 3023: 3019: 3016: 3012: 3009: 3005: 3002: 2998: 2994: 2990: 2986: 2982: 2978: 2974: 2970: 2968:9780199683208 2964: 2960: 2955: 2952: 2948: 2945: 2941: 2937: 2935:3-05-003732-6 2931: 2927: 2922: 2918: 2915: 2910: 2907: 2903: 2899: 2897:0-19-509427-1 2893: 2889: 2885: 2881: 2877: 2873: 2869: 2865: 2861: 2856: 2852: 2846: 2842: 2838: 2834: 2832: 2829: 2825: 2814: 2813: 2807: 2803: 2798: 2794: 2790: 2786: 2784:0-19-509308-9 2780: 2776: 2772: 2768: 2764: 2761: 2757: 2754: 2750: 2749: 2736: 2730: 2726: 2725: 2717: 2709: 2703: 2702:Pentland 2006 2698: 2691: 2686: 2678: 2674: 2670: 2666: 2662: 2658: 2651: 2644: 2638: 2631: 2626: 2624: 2617: 2616:0-914203-63-0 2613: 2607: 2600: 2595: 2588: 2583: 2576: 2571: 2564: 2559: 2552: 2547: 2540: 2535: 2528: 2523: 2516: 2511: 2504: 2499: 2492: 2487: 2480: 2475: 2469:, p. 41. 2468: 2463: 2456: 2451: 2444: 2439: 2432: 2427: 2420: 2415: 2408: 2403: 2396: 2391: 2385:, p. 61. 2384: 2379: 2373:, p. 72. 2372: 2367: 2360: 2355: 2348: 2343: 2336: 2331: 2325:, p. 39. 2324: 2323:Campbell 1997 2319: 2317: 2315: 2308:, 1954, p. 92 2307: 2301: 2297: 2284: 2278: 2274: 2263: 2262:Enindhilyagwa 2259: 2255: 2254:Murrinh-patha 2251: 2247: 2243: 2240: 2236: 2232: 2228: 2227: 2219: 2216: 2214: 2210: 2208: 2205: 2203: 2200: 2198: 2195: 2193: 2190: 2188: 2185: 2184: 2181:South America 2176: 2173: 2171: 2168: 2166: 2163: 2161: 2158: 2156: 2153: 2151: 2148: 2147: 2139: 2135: 2131: 2128: 2126: 2123: 2121: 2118: 2116: 2113: 2111: 2108: 2106: 2103: 2101: 2098: 2096: 2093: 2091: 2088: 2086: 2083: 2082: 2079:North America 2074: 2071: 2069: 2066: 2064: 2061: 2059: 2056: 2053: 2050: 2047: 2044: 2042: 2039: 2038: 2026: 2023: 2015: 2005: 2001: 1995: 1994: 1989:This section 1987: 1983: 1978: 1977: 1969: 1967: 1963: 1959: 1955: 1951: 1946: 1942: 1939: 1937: 1933: 1932:free morpheme 1926:Compositional 1923: 1921: 1917: 1913: 1909: 1905: 1901: 1893: 1889: 1885: 1881: 1880:scope ordered 1878: 1875: 1872: 1871: 1870: 1867: 1863: 1860: 1856: 1852: 1848: 1844: 1834: 1831: 1822: 1820: 1816: 1812: 1808: 1804: 1800: 1796: 1795:Mark C. Baker 1786: 1784: 1780: 1776: 1772: 1768: 1759: 1757: 1753: 1738: 1735: 1731: 1726: 1724: 1720: 1716: 1712: 1708: 1704: 1691: 1679: 1674: 1672: 1667: 1665: 1661: 1656: 1647: 1645: 1641: 1629: 1617: 1615: 1611: 1610:agglutination 1607: 1603: 1599: 1593: 1591: 1584:D. G. Brinton 1581: 1579: 1575: 1571: 1566: 1563: 1559: 1547: 1544: 1540: 1539:polysynthetic 1527: 1525: 1524:polysynthetic 1508: 1505: 1501: 1497: 1496:polysynthesis 1493: 1490: 1489: 1488: 1484: 1482: 1466: 1463: 1456: 1452: 1449: 1444: 1435: 1430: 1427: 1422: 1417: 1412: 1407: 1402: 1399: 1394: 1389: 1384: 1381: 1376: 1371: 1366: 1363: 1358: 1355: 1350: 1349:entertainment 1347: 1341: 1335: 1333: 1327: 1324: 1321: 1317: 1311: 1306: 1303: 1298: 1295: 1290: 1272: 1267: 1261: 1256: 1253: 1247: 1241: 1239: 1236: 1230: 1227: 1224: 1220: 1211: 1206: 1203: 1198: 1195: 1190: 1181: 1176: 1171: 1165: 1159: 1157: 1154: 1148: 1145: 1142: 1138: 1133: 1128: 1123: 1118: 1115: 1110: 1107: 1102: 1099: 1094: 1089: 1084: 1079: 1073: 1067: 1065: 1061: 1057: 1053: 1049: 1043: 1033: 1030: 1026: 1021: 1016: 1013: 1008: 1005: 1000: 995: 990: 987: 982: 977: 972: 967: 962: 957: 952: 947: 942: 939: 934: 931: 926: 923: 917: 915: 912: 908: 905: 900: 897: 892: 890: 884: 881: 878: 874: 865: 860: 857: 852: 849: 844: 841: 836: 827: 815: 813: 809: 805: 804:incorporating 801: 791: 789: 788:case stacking 785: 780: 775: 773: 769: 765: 764:incorporation 760: 758: 756: 750: 748: 742: 740: 734: 728: 726: 724: 718: 716: 710: 708: 702: 698: 696: 689: 684: 682: 678: 674: 669: 664: 662: 658: 647: 644: 636: 626: 622: 616: 615: 610:This section 608: 604: 599: 598: 590: 588: 584: 580: 579:Mark C. Baker 576: 572: 568: 564: 560: 556: 552: 547: 545: 541: 537: 536:evidentiality 533: 529: 523: 521: 517: 513: 512:agglutinative 508: 506: 502: 498: 494: 490: 486: 485:agglutinative 482: 478: 473: 470: 463: 460: 457: 453: 443: 438: 435: 430: 424: 419: 416: 411: 405: 400: 397: 392: 389: 383: 377: 374: 369: 365: 361: 357: 353: 350:, are highly 349: 345: 341: 329: 324: 322: 317: 315: 310: 309: 307: 306: 301: 298: 296: 293: 292: 291: 290: 287: 284: 283: 277: 275: 272: 270: 267: 265: 264:OS word order 262: 260: 259:V2 word order 257: 255: 254:V1 word order 252: 248: 245: 243: 240: 238: 235: 234: 233: 230: 226: 223: 221: 218: 216: 213: 212: 211: 208: 207: 206: 205: 202: 199: 198: 193: 190: 188: 185: 183: 180: 178: 175: 173: 170: 168: 165: 163: 162:Zero-marking 160: 154: 151: 149: 146: 145: 144: 141: 139: 136: 134: 131: 129: 126: 124: 121: 119: 116: 114: 111: 109: 106: 102: 99: 98: 97: 94: 93: 92: 89: 88: 87: 86: 83: 80: 79: 72: 69: 67: 66:Polysynthetic 64: 62: 61:Agglutinative 59: 57: 54: 53: 52: 49: 45: 42: 41: 40: 37: 36: 35: 34: 31: 30:Morphological 28: 27: 24: 21: 20: 3244: 3215: 3200: 3192: 3184: 3164: 3152: 3148: 3130: 3104: 3077: 3058: 3054: 3029: 3025: 3014: 3007: 3000: 2996: 2988: 2958: 2950: 2943: 2925: 2917: 2914: 2905: 2886:. New York: 2883: 2871: 2859: 2840: 2837:Blake, B. J. 2827: 2817:. Retrieved 2811: 2801: 2770: 2759: 2752: 2723: 2716: 2697: 2685: 2660: 2656: 2650: 2642: 2637: 2606: 2594: 2582: 2570: 2558: 2546: 2534: 2527:Jelinek 1984 2522: 2510: 2498: 2486: 2479:Brinton 1885 2474: 2467:Brinton 1885 2462: 2455:Whitney 1908 2450: 2438: 2426: 2421:, p. 3. 2414: 2407:Rowicka 2006 2402: 2390: 2378: 2366: 2354: 2342: 2330: 2305: 2300: 2277: 2018: 2009: 1998:Please help 1993:verification 1990: 1972:Distribution 1947: 1943: 1940: 1929: 1897: 1887: 1883: 1879: 1873: 1868: 1864: 1840: 1828: 1799:Noam Chomsky 1792: 1765: 1752:Noam Chomsky 1749: 1727: 1699: 1688: 1676: 1670: 1668: 1663: 1655:Edward Sapir 1653: 1637: 1626: 1595: 1587: 1567: 1555: 1542: 1538: 1536: 1523: 1520: 1495: 1486: 1478: 1464: 1457: 1454: 1450: 1436: 1428: 1418: 1408: 1400: 1390: 1382: 1372: 1364: 1356: 1348: 1329: 1325: 1322: 1319: 1312: 1304: 1296: 1273: 1262: 1254: 1232: 1228: 1225: 1222: 1212: 1204: 1202:rrurlimpirr- 1197:dead.wallaby 1196: 1182: 1172: 1150: 1146: 1143: 1140: 1134: 1124: 1116: 1108: 1100: 1090: 1080: 1045: 1034: 1031: 1028: 1022: 1014: 1006: 996: 988: 978: 968: 958: 948: 940: 932: 924: 916: 913: 910: 886: 882: 879: 876: 866: 858: 850: 842: 828: 797: 779:head marking 776: 771: 767: 761: 752: 744: 736: 729: 720: 712: 704: 692: 685: 665: 660: 656: 654: 639: 630: 619:Please help 614:verification 611: 583:Noam Chomsky 575:Edward Sapir 548: 524: 509: 501:head-marking 474: 465: 461: 458: 455: 444: 436: 425: 417: 406: 398: 390: 347: 343: 337: 232:OV languages 210:VO languages 182:Null-subject 177:Head-marking 65: 2443:Bieder 1986 2383:Suárez 1983 2359:Skorik 1961 2347:Payne (1997 2144:Mesoamerica 1964:, and most 1807:incorporate 1695:Sapir, 1921 1683:Sapir, 1921 1640:John Hewitt 1614:collocation 1565:evolution. 1383:one.good.at 1332:Greenlandic 1194:wuliyondji- 346:, formerly 295:Color terms 153:Indirective 148:Secundative 3257:Categories 3155:, 250–282. 3003:, 381–407. 2819:2013-01-14 2551:Baker 1996 2515:Sapir 1921 2503:Sapir 1921 2395:Blake 2006 2293:References 2218:Mapudungun 2136:and other 2054:(possible) 2048:(possible) 2012:April 2013 1918:) and the 1847:adjectives 1756:generative 1598:aphaeresis 1305:reversive- 1297:open.door- 571:New Guinea 540:mirativity 201:Word order 192:Theta role 128:Tripartite 3046:170296224 3032:: 39–76. 2977:967828955 2830:(Part 1). 2539:Hale 2003 2244:northern 1900:Inuktitut 1874:templatic 1811:Inuktitut 1644:Tuscarora 1543:syntactic 1504:Cape Horn 1500:Greenland 1460:SEMITRANS 1374:SEMITRANS 1109:something 899:Usaopuspe 707:nakupenda 701:Kiswahili 688:agreement 677:morphemes 673:morphemes 661:synthesis 633:July 2019 563:Australia 493:Hungarian 434:-ngqiggte 364:inflected 360:morphemes 91:Alignment 51:Synthetic 44:Isolating 2882:(1997). 2870:(1885). 2860:Language 2793:31045692 2769:(1996). 2657:Language 2645:, p. 337 1916:Quileute 1904:Cherokee 1793:In 1996 1771:Salishan 1719:Algonkin 1693:—  1681:—  1671:Language 1664:Language 1631:—  1619:—  1576:and the 1549:—  1529:—  1511:—  1429:sure.but 1401:say.that 1370:llammas- 1346:aliikku- 1233:And the 794:Examples 559:Americas 516:fusional 497:Georgian 475:Whereas 391:reindeer 300:Numerals 56:Fusional 39:Analytic 3268:Grammar 3236:8034800 2745:Sources 2349::28–29) 2224:Oceania 2035:Eurasia 1962:Tonkawa 1958:Chukchi 1851:adverbs 1837:Affixal 1819:Nahuatl 1715:Chinook 1606:apocope 1602:syncope 1426:galuar- 1357:provide 1271:nhoton- 1101:someone 1060:patient 1056:subject 1048:Nahuatl 925:various 800:Chukchi 772:babysit 593:Meaning 567:Siberia 538:and/or 423:-ksaite 286:Lexicon 3234:  3224:  3172:  3137:  3112:  3084:  3065:  3044:  2975:  2965:  2932:  2894:  2847:  2791:  2781:  2731:  2677:416217 2675:  2614:  2248:(e.g. 2233:(e.g. 1910:, the 1815:Mohawk 1711:Nootka 1398:nerar- 1354:sersu- 1260:honwa- 1255:again- 1238:Mohawk 1064:object 986:tuyma- 941:  938:  933:rumors 930:opuspe 896:ウサオプㇲペ 806:, and 569:, and 489:Basque 437:-again 404:-qatar 3205:Nauka 3042:S2CID 2673:JSTOR 2269:Notes 2239:Yimas 2235:Awtuw 2229:many 2211:Many 2052:Nivkh 1884:scope 1859:verbs 1855:nouns 1703:Haida 1574:Otomi 1434:paal- 1122:ltiː- 1114:maki- 1088:mits- 1007:heart 856:pəγt- 848:levt- 843:great 840:meyŋ- 755:tuntu 735:word 733:Yupik 703:word 695:hablo 544:verbs 520:affix 469:tuntu 399:-hunt 396:-ssur 388:tuntu 370:word 368:Yupik 356:words 3232:OCLC 3222:ISBN 3170:ISBN 3135:ISBN 3110:ISBN 3082:ISBN 3063:ISBN 2973:OCLC 2963:ISBN 2930:ISBN 2892:ISBN 2845:ISBN 2789:OCLC 2779:ISBN 2729:ISBN 2708:help 2612:ISBN 2134:Yahi 2130:Yana 2058:Ainu 1954:Sora 1950:Ainu 1857:and 1849:and 1817:and 1801:'s " 1773:and 1717:and 1709:and 1707:Yana 1612:and 1416:ssa- 1410:REFL 1380:sua- 1315:PERF 1313:for. 1281:> 1264:PAST 1156:Tiwi 1151:The 1126:CAUS 1117:give 1106:tla- 1098:teː- 1052:verb 1039:lit. 1024:ITER 1015:sway 1012:suy- 1004:ram- 998:REFL 970:REFL 966:yay- 922:usa- 889:Ainu 872:PRES 864:rkən 859:hurt 851:head 834:SUBJ 768:baby 668:word 657:poly 495:and 418:-say 3034:doi 2665:doi 2046:Ket 2002:by 1888:are 1754:'s 1541:or 1502:to 1451:but 1420:FUT 1406:ta- 1392:COP 1310:hse 1294:kw- 1252:sa- 1218:HAB 1214:PST 1210:ani 1180:ti- 1170:Pi- 1136:FUT 1078:ni- 994:si- 989:far 980:APL 976:ko- 960:APL 723:ku- 623:by 585:'s 530:or 514:or 451:IND 447:3SG 442:-uq 428:NEG 415:-ni 409:FUT 338:In 3259:: 3230:. 3153:13 3151:, 3059:86 3040:. 3028:. 2999:, 2971:. 2890:. 2787:. 2777:. 2671:. 2661:71 2659:. 2622:^ 2313:^ 2260:, 2256:, 2252:, 2237:, 1968:. 1960:, 1956:, 1952:, 1922:. 1906:, 1902:, 1604:, 1600:, 1448:li 1442:3S 1438:3P 1388:a- 1362:i- 1302:a- 1283:2S 1275:3S 1240:: 1184:3S 1174:3P 1092:2S 1082:1S 1062:, 1058:, 1020:pa 956:e- 950:1S 946:a- 868:1S 830:1S 826:t- 774:. 715:n- 565:, 561:, 546:. 522:. 507:. 491:, 376:. 342:, 3238:. 3207:. 3143:. 3118:. 3090:. 3071:. 3048:. 3036:: 3030:2 3001:6 2979:. 2953:. 2938:. 2900:. 2853:. 2822:. 2795:. 2762:. 2737:. 2710:) 2692:. 2679:. 2667:: 2553:. 2541:. 2529:. 2505:. 2433:. 2337:. 2285:. 2264:) 2241:) 2132:/ 2025:) 2019:( 2014:) 2010:( 1996:. 1914:( 1894:. 1616:. 1526:. 1506:. 1440:/ 1365:- 1289:- 1287:M 1285:. 1279:F 1277:. 1266:- 1216:. 1188:F 1186:. 1132:s 1037:( 870:. 832:. 646:) 640:( 635:) 631:( 617:. 449:. 445:- 426:- 407:- 327:e 320:t 313:v

Index

Linguistic typology
Morphological
Analytic
Isolating
Synthetic
Fusional
Agglutinative
Polysynthetic
Oligosynthetic
Morphosyntactic
Alignment
Nominative–accusative
Marked nominative
Ergative–absolutive
Split ergative
Symmetrical voice
Active–stative
Tripartite
Nominative–absolutive
Direct-inverse
Ditransitive/Monotransitive
Secundative
Indirective
Zero-marking
Dependent-marking
Double-marking
Head-marking
Null-subject
Syntactic pivot
Theta role

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.