Knowledge

Nominative–accusative alignment

Source 📝

1915:
this purpose, case can be used to mark one of the arguments. If one argument is case marked, this already suffices for the purpose of disambiguation. Thus, from the distinguishing perspective, there is no need to case mark both arguments. Neither would it be necessary to case mark the one and only argument of a one-place (intransitive) predicate. Indeed, it has been argued that in many nominative–accusative case systems only the y is case marked (with accusative case) while the x remains morphologically unmarked.
1612: 86: 36: 1938:
distinguishability seems to operate a principle of economy. It is more efficient to have as few cases as possible without compromising intelligibility. In this way the dual pressures of efficiency and economy have produced a system which patterns two kinds of arguments together a third separately. Both accusative and ergative systems use this kind of grouping to make meaning clearer.
679:
Nominative–accusative alignment can manifest itself in visible ways, called coding properties. Often, these visible properties are morphological and the distinction will appear as a difference in the actual morphological form and spelling of the word, or as case particles (pieces of morphology) which
1914:
When a two-place predicate R(x,y) is used to describe an event involving two participants, usually an agent and a patient, it is of utmost importance to avoid ambiguity as to which noun phrase corresponds to the first argument x (the agent) and which to the second argument y (the patient). For
1928:
One theory that has been posited to account for the occurrence of accusative systems is that of functional pressure. When applied to languages, this theory operates around the various needs and pressures on a speech community. It has been suggested that languages have evolved to suit the needs of
1619:
Languages exhibiting accusative alignment are the most widespread of all of the alignment types. These languages can be found on every continent, in comparison to languages with ergative alignment that are restricted to certain areas of the world, namely the Caucasus, parts of North American and
1481:
Nominative–accusative alignment can also be distinguished through behavioral properties, in the way a nominative or accusative argument will behave when placed in particular syntactic constructions. This has to do with the impact of alignment on the level of the whole sentence rather than the
1482:
individual word. Morphosyntactic alignment determines which arguments can be omitted in a coordinate structure during the process of conjunction reduction (deleting arguments from the ends of joined clauses). In nominative–accusative, only arguments S and A can be omitted and not argument O.
716:. It is highly common for only accusative arguments to exhibit overt case marking while nominative arguments exhibit null (or absent) case markings. In Modern English, case marking is only found with first and (non-neuter) third person pronouns, which have distinct subject and object forms. 1919:
It is rare for case to serve only the distinguishing function, which overlaps greatly with the ‘identify’ function. Other ways of disambiguating the arguments of a transitive predicate (subject agreement, word order restriction, context, intonation, etc.) may explain this cross-linguistic
1937:
alignment systems accomplish this differentiation by coding S, A and O all differently. However, this is not structurally economical, and tripartite systems are comparatively rare, but to have all arguments marked the same makes the arguments too ambiguous. Alongside the principle of
1134:
Not all arguments are equally likely to exhibit overt case marking. In languages with nominative–accusative alignment, it is common to divide direct objects into two classes (with respect to overt case marking), a phenomenon called ‘differential object marking’ by Bossong (1985).
1227:
In the following example from French, all subjects, both S and A, appear before the verb while O appears after the verb. Arguments occurring before the verb are coded as nominative, while arguments occurring directly after the verb are coded as accusative.
1143:
Some languages code very little through morphology and are more dependent on syntax to encode meaning and grammatical relationships. If a language relies less on overt case marking, alignment may be coded through word order, as in this example from
1929:
their users.These communities will develop some functional system to meet the needs that they have. So, it has been proposed that the accusative system arose from a functional pressure to avoid ambiguity and make communication a simpler process.
1620:
Mesoamerica, the Tibetan plateau, and Australia. The map shows the distribution of languages with the various alignment types, and the following list gives a short sampling of accusative languages and their distribution across the globe:
1889:
perspective. Case marking is said to fulfill two functions, or constraints: an identifying function and a distinguishing function. The identifying function is exemplified when case morphology encodes (identifies) specific
1932:
It is useful for languages to have a means of distinguishing between subjects and objects, and between arguments A, S, and O. This is helpful so that sentences like "Tom hit Fred" cannot be interpreted as "Fred hit Tom."
1920:
observation. De Hoop and Malchukov argue that case systems that are completely based on the identification function must be richer in case morphology compared to languages based mainly on the distinguishing function.
1550:
The omitted subject argument of the embedded clause must correspond to the subject (nominative) of the matrix-clause. If it corresponds to the object (accusative), the sentence is ungrammatical.
1588:
Here the omitted argument of the embedded clause corresponds to the direct object (absolutive) of the matrix-clause. If it corresponds to the subject (ergative), the sentence is ungrammatical.
444:. It has a wide global distribution and is the most common alignment system among the world's languages (including English). Languages with nominative–accusative alignment are commonly called 639:. In fact, there are relatively few languages that exhibit only ergative–absolutive alignment (called pure ergativity) and tend to be isolated in certain regions of the world, such as the 2141:
Fedzechkina, Maryia & Jaeger, T. Florian & Newport, Elissa L. (2011) "Functional Biases in Language Learning: Evidence from Word Order and Case-Marking Interaction".
2096:
Dryer, Matthew S. & Haspelmath, Martin (eds.). (2011) "The World Atlas of Language Structures Online". Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Available online at
399: 1907: 1898:
properties or information about the nominal argument. Accusative case in the position of the direct object, for example, can be a strong identifier of
17: 1473:
English has residual verb agreement with nominative–accusative alignment, which is only manifest with third person singular S and A in present tense.
54: 506:) argument of a transitive verb. English has nominative–accusative alignment in its case marking of personal pronouns: the single argument ( 432:, and are distinguished from objects of transitive verbs in basic clause constructions. Nominative–accusative alignment can be coded by 635:) to have overlapping alignment systems, which exhibit both nominative–accusative and ergative–absolutive coding, a phenomenon called 1902:. The distinguishing function is used to distinguish between the core arguments, the subject and the object, of a transitive clause. 474:. An intransitive verb is associated with only one argument, a subject. The different kinds of arguments are usually represented as 392: 320: 315: 310: 298: 293: 288: 155: 2052: 216: 385: 206: 169: 2027: 1999: 588: 72: 1960: 1885:
One of the ways in which the production of a nominative–accusative case marking system can be explained is from an
181: 50: 1326:
for S, A, and O. Both S in the intransitive clause and A in the transitive clause are marked by the same affix (
1318:
Alternatively, alignment can also manifest visibly through agreement on the verb. In the following example from
337: 2166: 211: 1129: 196: 305: 283: 240: 174: 1591:
The alignment system also impacts the triggering and realization of other such syntactic processes as
1965: 457: 421: 347: 164: 437: 327: 245: 144: 139: 134: 103: 1910:
explain the motivation and need for the distinguishing function in "Case marking strategies":
1600: 1596: 373: 255: 250: 235: 604: 226: 221: 201: 8: 2079:
Bickel, Balthasar; Iemmolo, Giorgio; Zakharko, Taras; Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena (2013).
1934: 1145: 413: 96: 688:
If a language exhibits morphological case marking, arguments S and A will appear in the
2016: 1858: 1742: 1662: 1656: 1323: 993: 632: 124: 117: 2048: 2023: 1995: 1899: 1886: 1852: 1803: 1797: 1718: 1693: 1687: 1631: 808: 660: 628: 503: 467: 425: 191: 129: 112: 1668: 1834: 1809: 1773: 1760: 1754: 1748: 1724: 1699: 1637: 1592: 1453: 1319: 921: 719: 701: 664: 636: 433: 186: 2070:
van de Visser, Mario. (2006) "The Marked Status of Ergativity". PhD. Dissertation.
2161: 1970: 1950: 1864: 1846: 1828: 1791: 1730: 1681: 1643: 1098: 1085: 1053: 1021: 999: 977: 936: 890: 874: 823: 709: 693: 689: 652: 495: 429: 342: 260: 700:. There may be more than one case fulfilling the accusative role; for instance, 1955: 1815: 1785: 1736: 705: 668: 2155: 1903: 1840: 1779: 1005: 644: 471: 463: 332: 1330:‘3SG.M’), while O in the transitive clause is marked by a different affix ( 697: 2111:
de Hoop, Helen and Malchukov, Andrej L. (2008) "Case-marking strategies".
1553:
If English were an ergative–absolutive language, one would expect to see:
1611: 648: 1895: 963:‘A/The (adolescent/youth) girl/young lady/young woman works/is working’ 674: 441: 368: 274: 265: 85: 1891: 656: 2080: 1041: 713: 640: 2085:
Languages across boundaries: Studies in memory of Anna Siewierska.
359: 462:
A transitive verb is associated with two noun phrases (or
1476: 451: 526:) of a transitive verb ("me" in the sentence “they saw 2097: 1123: 675:
Coding properties of nominative–accusative alignment
45:
may be too technical for most readers to understand
2015: 2153: 514:walked.") behaves grammatically like the agent ( 510:) of an intransitive verb ("I" in the sentence " 1994:, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 25, 1989: 1445: 1437: 1370: 1162: 764: 729: 490:is the sole argument of an intransitive verb, 1990:Donohue, Mark; Wichmann, Søren, eds. (2005), 522:saw them.") but differently from the object ( 518:) of a transitive verb ("I" in the sentence " 393: 1466:Lemma bottle-DEF-ACC break.PFV-3SG.M-3SG.M.O 789: 27:Concept of sentence structure in linguistics 1615:Distribution of languages by alignment type 2130:Language acquisition: The state of the art 1037:‘A/The student read/is reading a/the book’ 400: 386: 2107: 2105: 2081:"Patterns of alignment in verb agreement" 2042: 2013: 1433: 1366: 1180: 73:Learn how and when to remove this message 57:, without removing the technical details. 2128:. In E.Wanner, & L. Gleitman (Ed.), 1610: 1449: 1441: 1374: 84: 2132:. New York: Cambridge University Press. 2124:Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982) 2083:. In: Bakker, Dik; Haspelmath, Martin. 2045:An introduction to Japanese linguistics 2036: 1418: 1297: 1270: 1240: 1097: 1084: 1052: 1020: 998: 976: 935: 889: 873: 822: 14: 2154: 2102: 1983: 1923: 680:will appear before or after the word. 2066: 2064: 1477:Behavioral properties of accusativity 1176: 1004: 619:all are coded in a different manner. 498:) argument of a transitive verb, and 452:Comparison with other alignment types 55:make it understandable to non-experts 2118: 1880: 992: 627:It is common for languages (such as 29: 2135: 2126:Functionalist approaches to grammar 2090: 1597:subject-controlled subject deletion 1034:student-NOM read-3.SG.PRES book-ACC 911:Watashi-wa(S) kabin-wo(O) kowashita 793: 696:, or in a similar case such as the 622: 24: 2061: 1992:The Typology of Semantic Alignment 1875: 1601:object-controlled subject deletion 779: 744: 692:and argument O will appear in the 25: 2178: 2087:Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 15-36. 1313: 1124:Differential object marking (DOM) 960:(adolescent-/youth-)girl-NOM work 2047:. Wiley-Blackwell. p. 382. 1414: 34: 1606: 768: 733: 683: 446:nominative–accusative languages 418:nominative–accusative alignment 89:Nominative–accusative alignment 2073: 2022:. Cambridge University Press. 2007: 603:receives distinct marking, or 502:is the direct object (or most 18:Nominative–accusative language 13: 1: 2014:Van Valin, Robert D. (2001). 1976: 1138: 589:ergative–absolutive alignment 428:are treated like subjects of 1961:Ergative–absolutive language 595:is coded in the same way as 7: 2043:Tsujimura, Natsuko (2007). 1944: 1130:Differential object marking 1015: 987: 971: 946: 930: 900: 884: 868: 833: 817: 217:Ditransitive/Monotransitive 10: 2183: 1127: 1031:Studyent-Ø chitayet knig-u 455: 2018:An Introduction to Syntax 1966:Morphosyntactic alignment 1469:‘Lemma breaks the bottle’ 1459:Ləmma t’ərmus-u-n səbbər- 1220:1SG AT-wash-APPL man that 934:(adolescent-/youth-)girl- 659:. Such languages include 587:This is in contrast with 566: 552: 458:Morphosyntactic alignment 422:morphosyntactic alignment 1303:Je(A) jette un ballon(O) 1113:Vīrá-ḥ(S) áśva-m(O) ahan 802:1SG:SBJ see:PAST 3PL:OBJ 494:is the subject (or most 1028:Студент-Ø читает книг-у 704:marks objects with the 1917: 1684:– Bolivia, Chile, Peru 1616: 1306:I-NOM throw a ball-ACC 1119:‘The man slew a horse’ 1116:man-NOM horse-ACC slew 1012: 984: 968: 957:Dyevushka-Ø rabota-yet 943: 927: 897: 881: 865: 830: 814: 539:Nominative–accusative 90: 1912: 1614: 424:in which subjects of 207:Nominative–absolutive 170:Nominative–accusative 88: 1887:Optimality Theoretic 914:I-NOM vase-ACC broke 854:Kabin-ga(S) kowareta 605:tripartite alignment 542:Ergative–absolutive 2167:Linguistic typology 1924:Functional pressure 1217:-mandi-kan pria itu 1074:‘A horse was slain’ 954:Девушка-Ø работа-ет 414:linguistic typology 321:Object–verb–subject 316:Object–subject–verb 311:Subject–object–verb 299:Verb–object–subject 294:Verb–subject–object 289:Subject–verb–object 182:Ergative–absolutive 97:Linguistic typology 2113:Linguistic Inquiry 1617: 1386:Lemma go.PFV-3SG.M 1322:, the verb can be 1223:‘I bathe that man’ 917:‘I broke the vase’ 426:intransitive verbs 91: 2143:Cognitive Science 2054:978-1-4051-1065-5 1881:Optimality theory 1873: 1872: 1708: 1707: 1068:Áśva-ḥ(S) aghnata 753:1SG:SBJ walk:PAST 585: 584: 410: 409: 352:Place–manner–time 348:Time–manner–place 241:Dependent-marking 192:Symmetrical voice 175:Marked nominative 83: 82: 75: 16:(Redirected from 2174: 2146: 2139: 2133: 2122: 2116: 2109: 2100: 2094: 2088: 2077: 2071: 2068: 2059: 2058: 2040: 2034: 2033: 2021: 2011: 2005: 2004: 1987: 1908:Andrej Malchukov 1710: 1709: 1623: 1622: 1455: 1451: 1447: 1443: 1439: 1435: 1420: 1416: 1376: 1372: 1368: 1309:‘I throw a ball’ 1299: 1272: 1242: 1182: 1178: 1164: 1101: 1088: 1056: 1024: 1008: 1002: 996: 980: 939: 893: 877: 826: 795: 791: 781: 770: 766: 746: 735: 731: 665:Standard Tibetan 637:split ergativity 623:Split ergativity 533: 532: 430:transitive verbs 402: 395: 388: 93: 92: 78: 71: 67: 64: 58: 38: 37: 30: 21: 2182: 2181: 2177: 2176: 2175: 2173: 2172: 2171: 2152: 2151: 2150: 2149: 2140: 2136: 2123: 2119: 2110: 2103: 2095: 2091: 2078: 2074: 2069: 2062: 2055: 2041: 2037: 2030: 2012: 2008: 2002: 1988: 1984: 1979: 1971:Nominative case 1951:Accusative case 1947: 1941: 1926: 1894:, thematic, or 1883: 1878: 1876:Relevant theory 1677:South America: 1627:North America: 1609: 1595:constructions, 1584:was frightened. 1583: 1579: 1575: 1568: 1564: 1560: 1546:was frightened. 1545: 1541: 1537: 1531:was frightened. 1530: 1526: 1522: 1515: 1511: 1507: 1500: 1496: 1492: 1479: 1471: 1457: 1422: 1407: 1391: 1378: 1356: 1316: 1311: 1301: 1290: 1282: 1274: 1262: 1254:Je(S) travaille 1252: 1244: 1225: 1216: 1212: 1207: 1200: 1192: 1184: 1172: 1166: 1159: 1141: 1132: 1126: 1121: 1111: 1103: 1090: 1076: 1071:horse-NOM slain 1066: 1058: 1039: 1026: 1010: 982: 965: 952: 941: 919: 906: 895: 879: 862: 849: 839: 828: 806: 797: 783: 772: 757: 748: 737: 694:accusative case 690:nominative case 686: 677: 653:Tibetan Plateau 625: 460: 454: 436:-marking, verb 406: 343:Free word order 261:Syntactic pivot 156:Morphosyntactic 79: 68: 62: 59: 51:help improve it 48: 39: 35: 28: 23: 22: 15: 12: 11: 5: 2180: 2170: 2169: 2164: 2148: 2147: 2134: 2117: 2101: 2089: 2072: 2060: 2053: 2035: 2028: 2006: 2000: 1981: 1980: 1978: 1975: 1974: 1973: 1968: 1963: 1958: 1956:Case (grammar) 1953: 1946: 1943: 1925: 1922: 1882: 1879: 1877: 1874: 1871: 1870: 1869: 1868: 1862: 1856: 1850: 1844: 1838: 1832: 1821: 1820: 1819: 1813: 1807: 1801: 1795: 1789: 1783: 1777: 1766: 1765: 1764: 1758: 1752: 1746: 1740: 1734: 1728: 1722: 1706: 1705: 1704: 1703: 1697: 1691: 1685: 1674: 1673: 1672: 1666: 1660: 1649: 1648: 1647: 1641: 1635: 1608: 1605: 1586: 1585: 1581: 1577: 1573: 1570: 1566: 1562: 1558: 1548: 1547: 1543: 1539: 1535: 1532: 1528: 1524: 1520: 1517: 1513: 1509: 1505: 1502: 1498: 1494: 1490: 1478: 1475: 1423: 1408: 1400: 1399: 1398: 1397: 1357: 1349: 1348: 1347: 1346: 1340: 1339: 1315: 1314:Verb agreement 1312: 1291: 1283: 1275: 1264: 1263: 1245: 1234: 1233: 1214: 1210: 1201: 1193: 1185: 1170: 1167: 1157: 1154: 1153: 1140: 1137: 1128:Main article: 1125: 1122: 1104: 1091: 1078: 1077: 1059: 1046: 1045: 1011: 983: 967: 966: 942: 926: 925: 896: 880: 864: 863: 860:‘A vase broke’ 857:vase-NOM broke 840: 829: 813: 812: 784: 773: 759: 758: 738: 724: 723: 685: 682: 676: 673: 624: 621: 583: 582: 579: 576: 572: 571: 568: 565: 561: 560: 557: 554: 551: 547: 546: 543: 540: 537: 456:Main article: 453: 450: 408: 407: 405: 404: 397: 390: 382: 379: 378: 377: 376: 371: 363: 362: 356: 355: 354: 353: 350: 345: 340: 335: 330: 325: 324: 323: 318: 313: 303: 302: 301: 296: 291: 278: 277: 271: 270: 269: 268: 263: 258: 253: 248: 246:Double-marking 243: 238: 233: 232: 231: 230: 229: 224: 214: 212:Direct-inverse 209: 204: 199: 197:Active–stative 194: 189: 187:Split ergative 184: 179: 178: 177: 159: 158: 152: 151: 150: 149: 148: 147: 145:Oligosynthetic 142: 137: 132: 122: 121: 120: 107: 106: 100: 99: 81: 80: 42: 40: 33: 26: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 2179: 2168: 2165: 2163: 2160: 2159: 2157: 2144: 2138: 2131: 2127: 2121: 2114: 2108: 2106: 2099: 2093: 2086: 2082: 2076: 2067: 2065: 2056: 2050: 2046: 2039: 2031: 2029:9780521635660 2025: 2020: 2019: 2010: 2003: 2001:9780199238385 1997: 1993: 1986: 1982: 1972: 1969: 1967: 1964: 1962: 1959: 1957: 1954: 1952: 1949: 1948: 1942: 1939: 1936: 1930: 1921: 1916: 1911: 1909: 1905: 1904:Helen de Hoop 1901: 1897: 1893: 1888: 1866: 1863: 1860: 1857: 1854: 1851: 1848: 1845: 1842: 1839: 1836: 1833: 1830: 1827: 1826: 1825: 1822: 1817: 1814: 1811: 1808: 1805: 1802: 1799: 1796: 1793: 1790: 1787: 1784: 1781: 1778: 1775: 1772: 1771: 1770: 1767: 1762: 1759: 1756: 1753: 1750: 1747: 1744: 1741: 1738: 1735: 1732: 1729: 1726: 1723: 1720: 1717: 1716: 1715: 1712: 1711: 1701: 1698: 1695: 1692: 1689: 1686: 1683: 1680: 1679: 1678: 1675: 1671:– New Zealand 1670: 1667: 1664: 1661: 1658: 1655: 1654: 1653: 1652:Australasia: 1650: 1645: 1642: 1639: 1636: 1633: 1630: 1629: 1628: 1625: 1624: 1621: 1613: 1604: 1602: 1598: 1594: 1589: 1571: 1556: 1555: 1554: 1551: 1533: 1518: 1503: 1488: 1487: 1486: 1483: 1474: 1470: 1467: 1464: 1462: 1456: 1430: 1428: 1421: 1411: 1406: 1403: 1396: 1393: 1392: 1390: 1387: 1384: 1383: 1377: 1363: 1362: 1355: 1352: 1345: 1342: 1341: 1337: 1336: 1335: 1333: 1329: 1325: 1321: 1310: 1307: 1304: 1300: 1294: 1289: 1286: 1281: 1278: 1273: 1267: 1261: 1258: 1255: 1251: 1248: 1243: 1237: 1232: 1229: 1224: 1221: 1218: 1206: 1203: 1199: 1196: 1191: 1188: 1183: 1174: 1165: 1160: 1152: 1149: 1147: 1136: 1131: 1120: 1117: 1114: 1110: 1107: 1102: 1100: 1094: 1089: 1087: 1081: 1075: 1072: 1069: 1065: 1062: 1057: 1055: 1049: 1044: 1043: 1038: 1035: 1032: 1029: 1025: 1023: 1017: 1014: 1009: 1007: 1001: 995: 989: 986: 981: 979: 973: 970: 964: 961: 958: 955: 951: 948: 945: 940: 938: 932: 929: 924: 923: 918: 915: 912: 909: 905: 902: 899: 894: 892: 886: 883: 878: 876: 870: 869:Watashi-wa(S) 867: 861: 858: 855: 852: 848: 845: 842: 838: 835: 832: 827: 825: 819: 816: 811: 810: 805: 803: 800: 796: 787: 782: 776: 771: 762: 756: 754: 751: 747: 741: 736: 727: 722: 721: 717: 715: 711: 707: 703: 699: 695: 691: 681: 672: 670: 666: 662: 658: 654: 650: 646: 645:North America 642: 638: 634: 630: 620: 618: 614: 610: 606: 602: 598: 594: 590: 580: 577: 574: 573: 569: 563: 562: 558: 555: 549: 548: 544: 541: 538: 535: 534: 531: 529: 525: 521: 517: 513: 509: 505: 501: 497: 493: 489: 485: 481: 477: 473: 472:direct object 469: 465: 459: 449: 447: 443: 439: 435: 431: 427: 423: 420:is a type of 419: 415: 403: 398: 396: 391: 389: 384: 383: 381: 380: 375: 372: 370: 367: 366: 365: 364: 361: 358: 357: 351: 349: 346: 344: 341: 339: 338:OS word order 336: 334: 333:V2 word order 331: 329: 328:V1 word order 326: 322: 319: 317: 314: 312: 309: 308: 307: 304: 300: 297: 295: 292: 290: 287: 286: 285: 282: 281: 280: 279: 276: 273: 272: 267: 264: 262: 259: 257: 254: 252: 249: 247: 244: 242: 239: 237: 236:Zero-marking 234: 228: 225: 223: 220: 219: 218: 215: 213: 210: 208: 205: 203: 200: 198: 195: 193: 190: 188: 185: 183: 180: 176: 173: 172: 171: 168: 167: 166: 163: 162: 161: 160: 157: 154: 153: 146: 143: 141: 140:Polysynthetic 138: 136: 135:Agglutinative 133: 131: 128: 127: 126: 123: 119: 116: 115: 114: 111: 110: 109: 108: 105: 104:Morphological 102: 101: 98: 95: 94: 87: 77: 74: 66: 63:February 2016 56: 52: 46: 43:This article 41: 32: 31: 19: 2142: 2137: 2129: 2125: 2120: 2112: 2092: 2084: 2075: 2044: 2038: 2017: 2009: 1991: 1985: 1940: 1931: 1927: 1918: 1913: 1884: 1823: 1806:– Madagascar 1768: 1713: 1676: 1651: 1626: 1618: 1607:Distribution 1590: 1587: 1552: 1549: 1493:saw Judy-ACC 1484: 1480: 1472: 1468: 1465: 1460: 1458: 1431: 1426: 1424: 1412: 1409: 1404: 1401: 1394: 1389:‘Lemma came’ 1388: 1385: 1381: 1379: 1364: 1360: 1358: 1353: 1350: 1344:intransitive 1343: 1334:‘3SG.M.O’). 1331: 1327: 1317: 1308: 1305: 1302: 1295: 1292: 1287: 1284: 1279: 1276: 1268: 1265: 1259: 1256: 1253: 1249: 1246: 1238: 1235: 1230: 1226: 1222: 1219: 1208: 1204: 1202: 1197: 1194: 1189: 1186: 1175: 1168: 1161: 1155: 1150: 1142: 1133: 1118: 1115: 1112: 1108: 1105: 1095: 1092: 1082: 1079: 1073: 1070: 1067: 1063: 1060: 1050: 1047: 1040: 1036: 1033: 1030: 1027: 1018: 990: 974: 962: 959: 956: 953: 949: 933: 920: 916: 913: 910: 907: 903: 887: 871: 859: 856: 853: 850: 846: 843: 841: 836: 820: 807: 804: 801: 798: 788: 785: 777: 774: 763: 760: 755: 752: 749: 742: 739: 728: 725: 718: 712:to contrast 687: 684:Case marking 678: 626: 616: 612: 608: 600: 596: 592: 586: 527: 523: 519: 515: 511: 507: 504:patient-like 499: 491: 487: 483: 479: 475: 461: 445: 417: 411: 306:OV languages 284:VO languages 256:Null-subject 251:Head-marking 69: 60: 44: 1900:patienthood 1665:– Australia 1659:– Australia 1410:t’ərmus-u-n 1324:head-marked 1151:Indonesian 931:Dyevushka-Ø 885:kabin-wo(O) 818:Kabin-ga(S) 799:I saw them. 649:Mesoamerica 643:, parts of 545:Tripartite 369:Color terms 227:Indirective 222:Secundative 2156:Categories 1977:References 1935:Tripartite 1861:– Mongolia 1831:– Pakistan 1818:– Ethiopia 1788:– Tanzania 1690:– Colombia 1489:a. Sue-NOM 1395:transitive 1380:Ləmma hed- 1257:I-NOM work 1173:-mandi-kan 1146:Indonesian 1139:Word order 972:Studyent-Ø 947:rabota-yet 908:私は 花瓶を 壊した 710:accusative 633:Hindustani 581:different 570:different 559:different 496:agent-like 442:word order 275:Word order 266:Theta role 202:Tripartite 1896:pragmatic 1859:Mongolian 1837:– Myanmar 1800:– Namibia 1782:– Nigeria 1776:– Morocco 1745:– Hungary 1743:Hungarian 1733:– Germany 1727:– Finland 1721:– Armenia 1696:– Ecuador 1663:Mangarayi 1657:Kayardild 1580:, and ___ 1565:, and ___ 1527:, and she 1497:, and she 1293:ballon(O) 1247:travaille 1093:áśva-m(O) 1080:Vīrá-ḥ(S) 1048:Áśva-ḥ(S) 969:Студент-Ø 944:работа-ет 928:Девушка-Ø 901:kowashita 750:I walked. 706:partitive 657:Australia 578:different 556:different 464:arguments 438:agreement 165:Alignment 125:Synthetic 118:Isolating 1945:See also 1892:semantic 1853:Japanese 1849:– Israel 1804:Malagasy 1798:Khoekhoe 1769:Africa: 1757:– Russia 1751:– Turkey 1739:– Greece 1719:Armenian 1714:Europe: 1702:– Brazil 1688:Barasana 1632:Cahuilla 1576:saw Judy 1561:saw Judy 1538:saw Judy 1523:saw Judy 1508:saw Judy 1485:English 1260:‘I work’ 1042:Sanskrit 988:chitayet 975:student- 834:kowareta 809:Japanese 714:telicity 661:Sumerian 641:Caucasus 629:Georgian 607:, where 599:, while 591:, where 374:Numerals 130:Fusional 113:Analytic 1867:– Korea 1855:– Japan 1843:– India 1835:Burmese 1812:– Sudan 1763:– Spain 1761:Spanish 1755:Russian 1749:Turkish 1725:Finnish 1700:Xavante 1694:Quechua 1638:Koasati 1593:raising 1572:c’. Sue 1557:b’. Sue 1542:and ___ 1512:and ___ 1425:səbbər- 1413:bottle- 1338:Amharic 1320:Amharic 1231:French 1061:aghnata 922:Russian 851:花瓶が 壊れた 740:walked. 720:English 708:or the 702:Finnish 698:oblique 536:  468:subject 440:and/or 360:Lexicon 49:Please 2162:Syntax 2051:  2026:  1998:  1865:Korean 1847:Hebrew 1829:Brahui 1824:Asia: 1810:Nubian 1794:– Chad 1792:Kanuri 1774:Berber 1731:German 1682:Aymara 1646:– U.S. 1640:– U.S. 1634:– U.S. 1534:d. Sue 1519:c. Sue 1504:b. Sue 1432:break. 1205:  1179:-wash- 1096:horse- 1051:horse- 1016:knig-u 1013:книг-у 985:читает 847:  844:  667:, and 655:, and 651:, the 482:, and 470:and a 1816:Oromo 1786:Iraqw 1737:Greek 1669:Māori 1644:Miwok 1405:Lemma 1402:Ləmma 1354:Lemma 1351:Ləmma 1296:ball- 1280:throw 1277:jette 1266:Je(A) 1236:Je(S) 1064:slain 1019:book- 991:read- 904:broke 888:vase- 837:broke 821:vase- 786:them. 743:walk: 669:Mayan 530:."). 466:): a 2098:WALS 2049:ISBN 2024:ISBN 1996:ISBN 1906:and 1841:Garo 1780:Igbo 1599:and 1582:*i/j 1569:ran. 1567:*i/j 1544:i/*j 1516:ran. 1514:i/*j 1501:ran. 1359:hed- 1250:work 1209:saya 1198:that 1187:pria 1181:APPL 1156:saya 1109:slew 1106:ahan 1083:man- 1006:PRES 950:work 780:PAST 778:see: 745:PAST 647:and 631:and 615:and 567:same 553:same 434:case 1529:i/j 1499:i/j 1446:3SG 1438:3SG 1434:PFV 1419:ACC 1415:DEF 1371:3SG 1367:PFV 1365:go. 1298:ACC 1271:NOM 1241:NOM 1195:itu 1190:man 1163:1SG 1099:ACC 1086:NOM 1054:NOM 1022:ACC 978:NOM 937:NOM 898:壊した 891:ACC 882:花瓶を 875:NOM 831:壊れた 824:NOM 815:花瓶が 794:OBJ 790:3PL 775:saw 769:SBJ 765:1SG 734:SBJ 730:1SG 412:In 53:to 2158:: 2104:^ 2063:^ 1603:. 1463:-w 1429:-w 1332:-w 1328:-ə 1285:un 1269:I- 1239:I- 1213:me 1177:AT 1169:me 1148:. 1000:SG 872:I- 866:私は 671:. 663:, 611:, 575:O 564:S 550:A 528:me 486:. 478:, 448:. 416:, 2145:. 2115:. 2057:. 2032:. 1578:j 1574:i 1563:j 1559:i 1540:j 1536:i 1525:j 1521:i 1510:j 1506:i 1495:j 1491:i 1461:ə 1454:O 1452:. 1450:M 1448:. 1444:- 1442:M 1440:. 1436:- 1427:ə 1417:- 1382:ə 1375:M 1373:. 1369:- 1361:ə 1288:a 1215:i 1211:i 1171:i 1158:i 1003:. 997:. 994:3 792:: 767:: 761:I 732:: 726:I 617:O 613:S 609:A 601:A 597:O 593:S 524:O 520:I 516:A 512:I 508:S 500:O 492:A 488:S 484:O 480:A 476:S 401:e 394:t 387:v 76:) 70:( 65:) 61:( 47:. 20:)

Index

Nominative–accusative language
help improve it
make it understandable to non-experts
Learn how and when to remove this message

Linguistic typology
Morphological
Analytic
Isolating
Synthetic
Fusional
Agglutinative
Polysynthetic
Oligosynthetic
Morphosyntactic
Alignment
Nominative–accusative
Marked nominative
Ergative–absolutive
Split ergative
Symmetrical voice
Active–stative
Tripartite
Nominative–absolutive
Direct-inverse
Ditransitive/Monotransitive
Secundative
Indirective
Zero-marking
Dependent-marking

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.