Knowledge

Iconicity

Source 📝

855:). Some proponents believe that iconicity does not play an actual role in perception and production of signs once they have undergone phonological reduction and become part of the conventionalized vocabulary. More recently, the possible role of iconicity is being evaluated again. Current research on sign language phonology acknowledges that certain aspects are semantically motivated. Further, the ability to modify sign meaning through phonological changes to signs is gaining attention. The ability to work creatively with sign language in this way has been associated with accomplished, or native signers. 924:
reader must travel a great distance across the poem, therefore, in order to "arrive". The spatial dimension, then, can relate to a temporal dimension. In the poems "The Fish" and "The Moose" by Elizabeth Bishop, temporal iconicity is at work. The amount of time it takes to read "The Fish" coincides with the length of time a fish could live outside of water; likewise, the duration of the long bus ride in "The Moose" coincides with the poem's long first sentence as well as the twenty-some stanzas it takes before the passengers on the bus (and the reader) actually encounters the moose.
29: 1102:
tongue recedes in articulating vowels from the front to the back of the mouth, and as acoustic frequencies become lower, the vowels are judged to be larger and darker". Bentley and Varron (1933) ran tests asking subjects to differentiate between vowel sounds without providing them, beforehand, contrasting attributes (such as bright and dark.) They found only moderate success rates that decreased when vowel sounds were closer in tone. However, they still found that sounds were judged larger or lower than sounds.
902:). Today it is often recognized that sign languages exhibit a greater degree of iconicity compared to spoken languages due to the visual natural of sign languages. However, the structure of sign languages also puts limits to the degree of iconicity: From a truly iconic language one would expect that a concept like SMILING would be expressed by mimicking a smile (i.e., by performing a smiling face). All known sign languages, however, do not express the concept of SMILING by a smiling face, but by a manual sign. 514: 866:(ASL) a grammatical marker denoting “intensity” is characterized by a movement pattern with two parts: an initial pause, followed by a quick completion. When this pattern is added to the adjective GOOD the resulting meaning is VERY-GOOD. The ASL marker for "intensity" is iconic in that the intended meaning (building of pressure, a sudden release) is matched by the articulatory form (a pause, a quick completion). 711:, with the indexical signs of animal communication systems having no capacity for displacement, and the symbolic signs of human language requiring it. Iconic signs, however, "may or may not have it depending on how they’re used ... iconicity, therefore, is the most probable road that our ancestors took into language". 751:
is iconic with the world beyond language signs, and endophoric where the signifier is iconic to another signifier within language. By endophoric he does not mean "trivial" recurrences like the letter 'e' in one sentence being iconic with the letter 'e' in another sentence, which are not iconic signs
869:
Like in vocal languages, developmental trends in ASL shy away from iconicity in favor of arbitrariness. These changes "contribute toward symmetry, fluidity, locational displacement and assimilation". For example, the sign WE used to contain the sign for each individual being described by the WE. So
633:
is quoted, “The process is generally employed, with self-evident symbolism, to indicate such concepts as distribution, plurality, repetition, customary activity, increase of size, added intensity, continuance” (1921:79). This has been confirmed by the comparative studies of Key (1965) and Moravcsik
763:
icon refers back to earlier of its recurrences in the text and the traces of them in our memory, it is an iconic sign. Insofar as these morphemes constitute a coherent pattern of relations which create a line of mentation, they form a diagrammatic icon". Intertextual iconicity would include things
735:
argues that the emanation of symbolic capacities unique to language was a critical factor in the evolution of the human brain, and that these symbolic capacities are vital to differentiating animal from human forms of communication, processes of learning, and brain anatomy. "The doorway into this
923:
A subset of visual iconicity involves a spatial iconicity. For instance, in Cummings's grasshopper poem ("r-p-o-p-h-e-s-s-a-g-r") the word "arriving" begins on the far right of the poem with the "a", the "r" is near the middle of the poem, and the rest of the word is on the left of the poem. The
1101:
asked subjects to differentiate between two different sized tables using invented word pairs such as "mal" and "mil". He discovered a word containing was at four times more likely to be judged as larger if paired with a word containing . Nuckolls states: "Newman discovered that ... as the
1013:
It has been suggested that iconicity can be used in the teaching of languages. There are two ways this has been suggested. The first being “Horizontal-Iconicity” and the second being vowel magnitude relationships. Horizontal-Iconicity is the phenomenon of opposition of meaning and spelling. For
885:
On the whole, some researchers stress that iconicity plays an important role in sign languages, while others downplay its role. The reason for this also lies in the fact that it was, for a long time, assumed that it is a major property of natural languages that there is no relation between the
910:
Iconicity often occurs within poetry through the use of onomatopoeia, which may be called auditory iconicity. Sometimes the form of the poem resembles or enacts the poem's content, and in this case, a visual iconicity is present. One poet well known for his visual poems, and therefore visual
722:. This process "would have created new words and deployed old words in new contexts, further weakening the uncoupling of words from situations, from current occurrence—even from fitness", and thus allowing for the creation of symbolic language. 827:, described by Savage-Rumbaugh et al., who could indicate direction of travel by "extending his hand". Another chimpanzee, Viki (Hayes and Nissen 1971:107) made motions of kneading or ironing when she wanted to knead dough or iron napkins. 898:. Thus, many linguists concerned with sign languages tried to downplay the role of iconicity in sign languages. It was, however, later acknowledged that iconicity also plays a role in many spoken languages (see, for example 802:
Iconic calls and gestures mimic the forms of the things they stand for (such as outlining shapes or moving your hands back and forth multiple times to show repetition.) Iconic calls and gestures are not formally considered
1000:
as having deep intrinsic truth, which might have been religiously and homiletically based. One should not forget that at that time it was a common belief that all languages were God-created and that Hebrew was the divine
707:(and, occasionally, iconic) signs, whereas in human language, "most words are symbolic, and ... without symbolic words we couldn’t have language". The distinction Bickerton draws between these categories is one of 736:
virtual world was opened to us alone by the evolution of language, because language is not merely a mode of communication, it is also the outward expression of an unusual mode of thought—symbolic representation."
677:
has argued against iconicity, claiming that most iconic phenomena can be explained by frequency biases: since simpler meanings tend to be more frequent in the language use they tend to lose phonological material.
1428:
For the history of research on iconicity in sign languages see, for example: Vermeerbergen, Myriam (2006): Past and current trends in sign language research. In: Language & Communication, 26(2). 168-192.
624:
The use of quantity of phonetic material to iconically mark increased quality or quantity can be noted in the lengthening of words to indicate a greater degree, such as "looong". It is also common to use
1418:
Hocket proposed 13 features which a natural language should have to count as a natural language. See feature number 8 in: Hockett, Charles (1960): The Origin of Speech. in Scientific American, 203, pp.
1094:. Open vowel sounds are also more likely to be associated with round shapes and dark or gloomy moods, where closed vowel sounds are more likely to be associated with pointed shapes and happy moods. 1761:
Moravcsik, Edith, A. 1978. Reduplicative constructions. In Universals of human language, vol. 3: Word structure, Joseph H. Greenberg, ed., 297–334. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
1117:
cites examples of word order mimicking the natural order of ideas. In fact, iconicity is now widely acknowledged to be a significant factor at many levels of linguistic structure.
1607: 1109:, examples from degree adjectives, such as long, longer, longest, show that the most extreme degree of length is iconically represented by the word with the greatest number of 882:+ ME. Now the sign has turned into a smooth symbolic sign where the signer makes two touches on the chest, one on each side, with a sweep of the wrist in between. 807:, or language-like communication in that they do not contrast or possess arbitrary characteristics. Noises that imitate sounds of the surrounding environment ( 759:. An example of intratextual endophoric iconicity is "the various recurrences of the word icon and its derivatives iconic or iconicity....Insofar as the 985: 1487: 718:
view of human evolution, Bickerton has hypothesized that human ancestors used iconic signs as recruitment signals in the scavenging of dead
919:. In his poem "A Wreath" (1633) each line overlaps the next while the rhyme scheme makes a circle, thus mimicking the form of a wreath. 1142: 1437:
Bross, Fabian (2020). The clausal syntax of German Sign Language. A cartographic approach. Berlin: Language Science Press. Page 25.
1310:
Burling, Robbins (February 1993). "Primate Calls, Human Language, and Nonverbal Communication [and Comments and Reply]".
823:
states: "Chimpanzees in the wild do not point, and rarely do so in captivity, however there is a documented case of one named
1642: 1270: 541: 431: 688:
more broadly) may be seen as a kind of iconicity, though even onomatopoeic sounds have a large degree of arbitrariness.
1482:' and the Power of 'Lexical Engineering' in Judaism, Islam and Christianity. A Socio-Philo(sopho)logical Perspective", 1072: 996:
explanation for the pogrom was regarded by some Jews as a mere play on words. However, others might have conceived of
1235: 72: 50: 1041:
Vowel magnitude relationships suggest that, the larger the object, the more likely its name has open vowels such as
43: 1456: 1199: 1056: 391: 1798: 1371:
Frishberg, Nancy (September 1975). "Arbitrariness and Iconicity: Historical Change in American Sign Language".
451: 396: 169: 426: 117: 371: 237: 1050: 699:
has posited that iconic signs, both verbal and gestural, were crucial in the evolution of human language.
1064: 848: 491: 197: 1629:. Interactive Technologies (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers. pp. 854–866. 1033:
tags for, it is suggested that it may be helpful to point these things out in the teaching of language.
886:
surface form of a word and its potential referents (thus, there is no relationship between how the word
851:. In sign languages iconicity was often argued to be largely confined to sign formation (comparable to 708: 1768:
Wilcox, S (2004). "Conceptual spaces and embodied actions: Cognitive iconicity and signed languages".
1574:
Croft, L. B. (1978). "The Mnemonic Use of Linguistic Iconicity in Teaching Language and Literature".
1070: 1062: 1054: 1042: 894:). The idea that iconicity should not play a role in natural languages was, for example, stressed by 534: 481: 381: 207: 615:
Sequential order principle: the sequential order of events described is mirrored in the speech chain
1194: 1106: 899: 784: 386: 329: 144: 37: 1162: 1044: 933: 673:
make-up. Whether iconicity is a part of language is an open debate in linguistics. For instance,
486: 324: 301: 1132: 863: 862:
called classifiers. These are used to give descriptive information about a subject or verb. In
595: 587: 436: 403: 356: 272: 252: 232: 134: 112: 107: 54: 988:
1991: 42). Thus, if a pogrom had occurred in Radom, it would surely have been rationalized by
1603: 1472: 1406: 1262: 1253: 591: 579: 555: 212: 1764:
Shapiro, Bruce G. 1999. Reinventing Drama: Acting, Iconicity, Performance. Greenwood Press.
832: 727: 700: 527: 456: 366: 247: 192: 89: 1291:
NĂśth, Winfried (Summer 1999). "Peircean Semiotics in the Study of Iconicity in Language".
1029:, which means left hand. Because people are more likely to remember things they have more 811:) are also iconic. Though humans possess a repertoire of iconic calls and gestures, other 791: 8: 771:
Specific utterances which adhere to the rules of a language are iconic with one another.
599: 598:
approaches to linguistics). The principle of iconicity is also shared by the approach of
297: 227: 202: 174: 1803: 1743: 1714: 1685: 1634: 1591: 1388: 1335: 1327: 1127: 715: 517: 496: 466: 421: 376: 344: 334: 222: 217: 1656: 1648: 1638: 1491: 1452: 1339: 1266: 1231: 1015: 937: 816: 513: 361: 339: 282: 1532: 1777: 1735: 1706: 1677: 1668:
Frishberg, N. (1975). "Arbitrariness and Iconicity: Historical Change in America".
1630: 1583: 1562: 1528: 1380: 1319: 1152: 1080: 1061:; the smaller the object, the more likely its name has closed vowel sounds such as 968:, the name of a town in Poland (approximately 100 kilometres (62 mi) south of 843:
Iconicity is often argued to play a large role in the production and perception of
783:. Another example is “the relationship between great, greater, greatest….since the 635: 575: 461: 292: 287: 262: 257: 242: 744: 1618: 981: 960: 952: 895: 732: 696: 612:
Proximity principle: conceptual distance tends to match with linguistic distance
567: 1614: 1476: 1114: 993: 916: 912: 859: 306: 1660: 747:
distinguishes between endophoric and exophoric iconicity, exophoric where the
1792: 1697:
Haiman, John (1980). "The Iconicity of Grammar: Isomorphism and Motivation".
1157: 891: 626: 583: 1098: 852: 828: 756: 704: 681: 630: 349: 139: 1781: 1566: 959:'of bad blood'). This is a toponymic rejective phono-semantic matching of 609:
Quantity principle: conceptual complexity corresponds to formal complexity
1622: 501: 476: 97: 1550: 1331: 1200:
Some Contributions of Typology to Cognitive Linguistics, and Vice Versa
674: 471: 154: 1747: 1718: 1689: 1595: 1392: 1259:
The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Human Brain
1147: 890:
is pronounced and what a computer, for example, looks like, see also
808: 788: 776: 755:
Textual endophoric iconicity can be divided between intratextual and
748: 719: 666: 559: 441: 277: 267: 159: 149: 1739: 1710: 1681: 1608:"Media Streams: An Iconic Visual Language for Video Representation" 1587: 1479: 1384: 1323: 1137: 1030: 804: 780: 765: 760: 1627:
Readings in Human–Computer Interaction: Toward the Year 2000
1228:
Adam's Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans
1110: 973: 945: 844: 772: 685: 670: 662: 571: 1652: 1451:. The Missionary Society of St. Paul the Apostle. p. 312. 969: 812: 775:
can also be iconic with one another in that they could both be
164: 1514: 1512: 1086: 964: 824: 1519:
Nuckolls, Janis B. (1999). "The Case for Sound Symbolism".
1509: 1230:. New York, NY: Hill & Wang. pp. 52–53, 218–222. 847:. Proposed ways in which iconicity is achieved is through 1205:
Cognitive Linguistics: Foundations, Scope and Methodology
1083: 815:
produce few iconic signals. Despite this, a few captive
1726:
Haiman, John (1983). "Iconic and Economic Motivation".
858:
Iconicity is expressed in the grammatical structure of
703:
systems, Bickerton has argued, are largely composed of
1484:
Explorations in the Sociology of Language and Religion
1465: 1181:
Luraghi, S. (2010) Introduzione, in Crof & Cruise
1024: 1018: 819:
have shown the beginning stages of iconicity. Burling
1252: 1754:Hinton, L., Nichols, J., and Ohala, J.J. (1994). 1790: 665:tendencies in language and are also part of our 1219: 1449:George Herbert: The Country Parson, The Temple 1502: 1500: 1293:Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society 992:‘of bad blood’. Obviously, providing such an 648: 640: 535: 1244: 691: 739: 1497: 1446: 1303: 915:. Another poet known for "shape poems" is 794:is the same as in loud, louder, loudest”. 634:(1978). This can be seen, for example, in 542: 528: 1667: 1494:, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 237–258. 1370: 1318:(1). University of Chicago Press: 30–31. 1225: 1143:Semiotic theory of Charles Sanders Peirce 73:Learn how and when to remove this message 1758:. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1518: 1261:. Allen Lane the Penguin Press. p.  36:This article includes a list of general 1309: 1203:, in Janssen, Th and G. Redeker (1999) 1008: 849:Hands that Act, Embody, Model, and Draw 1791: 1767: 1725: 1696: 1250: 1602: 1573: 1548: 797: 619: 1576:The Slavic and East European Journal 1290: 1286: 1284: 1282: 831:are another example of iconicity in 432:Conservative and innovative language 22: 18:Aspect of linguistics and semiotics 13: 1635:10.1016/B978-0-08-051574-8.50089-3 1405:This idea is often traced back to 1036: 752:of one another according to NĂśth. 578:(linguistic or otherwise) and its 42:it lacks sufficient corresponding 14: 1815: 1279: 927: 838: 1079: 661:Iconic coding principles may be 629:to iconically mark increase, as 512: 27: 1623:Buxton, William A. S. 1555:Sign Language & Linguistics 1542: 1533:10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.225 1440: 1431: 1422: 1412: 1399: 1364: 658:means that "it was shattered". 586:(which is typically assumed in 1355: 1346: 1210: 1188: 1175: 932:Iconicity occurs in rejective 870:the signer would sign ME + YOU 1: 1521:Annual Review of Anthropology 1169: 1023:, which means right hand and 951:), lit. 'of bad blood' (from 1447:Wall (Jr.), John N. (1984). 833:animal communication systems 372:Functional discourse grammar 238:Ethnography of communication 7: 1185:, Italian edition, pp.19-20 1120: 492:Second-language acquisition 10: 1820: 1625:; Greenberg, Saul (eds.). 646:means "it was broken" and 170:Syntax–semantics interface 1251:Deacon, Terrence (1997). 1226:Bickerton, Derek (2009). 905: 692:The evolution of language 482:Philosophy of linguistics 382:Interactional linguistics 900:Japanese sound symbolism 740:Endophoric and exophoric 1025: 1019: 936:. "Consider Lithuanian 934:phono-semantic matching 57:more precise citations. 1133:Charles Sanders Peirce 864:American Sign language 649: 641: 574:between the form of a 319:Theoretical frameworks 273:Philosophy of language 253:History of linguistics 1799:Cognitive linguistics 1782:10.1515/cogl.2004.005 1770:Cognitive Linguistics 1567:10.1075/sll.22003.bro 1407:Ferdinand de Saussure 1183:Linguistica cognitiva 556:cognitive linguistics 213:Conversation analysis 1551:"What is iconicity?" 1312:Current Anthropology 1009:Language acquisition 728:The Symbolic Species 701:Animal communication 457:Internet linguistics 367:Construction grammar 1473:Zuckermann, Ghil‘ad 605:Iconic principles: 600:linguistic typology 392:Systemic functional 187:Applied linguistics 129:General linguistics 1615:Baecker, Ronald M. 1549:Bross, F. (2024). 1128:Autonomy of syntax 798:Calls and gestures 768:, quotations etc. 716:niche-construction 620:Quantity principle 497:Theory of language 467:Origin of language 422:Autonomy of syntax 377:Grammaticalization 223:Discourse analysis 218:Corpus linguistics 1644:978-1-55860-246-5 1492:Joshua A. Fishman 1352:Frishburg (1975). 1272:978-0-393-03838-5 938:Ashkenazic Hebrew 881: 877: 873: 566:is the conceived 552: 551: 340:Distributionalism 283:Psycholinguistics 83: 82: 75: 1811: 1785: 1751: 1722: 1693: 1664: 1619:Grudin, Jonathan 1612: 1599: 1570: 1537: 1536: 1527:: 230–231, 246. 1516: 1507: 1504: 1495: 1469: 1463: 1462: 1444: 1438: 1435: 1429: 1426: 1420: 1416: 1410: 1403: 1397: 1396: 1368: 1362: 1359: 1353: 1350: 1344: 1343: 1307: 1301: 1300: 1288: 1277: 1276: 1256: 1248: 1242: 1241: 1223: 1217: 1216:Moravcsik (1978) 1214: 1208: 1192: 1186: 1179: 1163:Triadic relation 1093: 1092: 1089: 1088: 1085: 1076: 1068: 1060: 1052: 1048: 1028: 1022: 879: 875: 871: 656: 644: 582:, as opposed to 558:, as well as in 544: 537: 530: 516: 462:LGBT linguistics 452:Internationalism 427:Compositionality 288:Sociolinguistics 263:Neurolinguistics 258:Interlinguistics 243:Ethnomethodology 85: 84: 78: 71: 67: 64: 58: 53:this article by 44:inline citations 31: 30: 23: 1819: 1818: 1814: 1813: 1812: 1810: 1809: 1808: 1789: 1788: 1756:Sound Symbolism 1645: 1610: 1545: 1540: 1517: 1510: 1505: 1498: 1477:Etymythological 1470: 1466: 1459: 1445: 1441: 1436: 1432: 1427: 1423: 1417: 1413: 1404: 1400: 1369: 1365: 1360: 1356: 1351: 1347: 1308: 1304: 1289: 1280: 1273: 1249: 1245: 1238: 1224: 1220: 1215: 1211: 1193: 1189: 1180: 1176: 1172: 1167: 1123: 1082: 1078: 1039: 1037:Vowel magnitude 1011: 994:etymythological 982:Uriel Weinreich 930: 922: 908: 896:Charles Hockett 841: 800: 742: 733:Terrence Deacon 697:Derek Bickerton 694: 622: 548: 507: 506: 417: 409: 408: 320: 312: 311: 307:Writing systems 198:Anthropological 188: 180: 179: 130: 122: 79: 68: 62: 59: 49:Please help to 48: 32: 28: 19: 12: 11: 5: 1817: 1807: 1806: 1801: 1787: 1786: 1776:(2): 119–147. 1765: 1762: 1759: 1752: 1740:10.2307/413373 1734:(4): 781–819. 1723: 1711:10.2307/414448 1705:(3): 515–540. 1694: 1682:10.2307/412894 1676:(3): 696–719. 1665: 1643: 1600: 1588:10.2307/307673 1582:(4): 509–518. 1571: 1544: 1541: 1539: 1538: 1508: 1496: 1471:See p. 246 of 1464: 1457: 1439: 1430: 1421: 1411: 1398: 1385:10.2307/412894 1379:(3): 696–719. 1363: 1354: 1345: 1324:10.1086/204132 1302: 1278: 1271: 1243: 1236: 1218: 1209: 1187: 1173: 1171: 1168: 1166: 1165: 1160: 1155: 1150: 1145: 1140: 1135: 1130: 1124: 1122: 1119: 1097:A test run by 1038: 1035: 1010: 1007: 929: 928:Folk etymology 926: 917:George Herbert 913:E. E. Cummings 911:iconicity, is 907: 904: 860:sign languages 840: 839:Sign languages 837: 799: 796: 741: 738: 693: 690: 621: 618: 617: 616: 613: 610: 554:In functional- 550: 549: 547: 546: 539: 532: 524: 521: 520: 509: 508: 505: 504: 499: 494: 489: 487:Prescriptivism 484: 479: 474: 469: 464: 459: 454: 449: 444: 439: 434: 429: 424: 418: 415: 414: 411: 410: 407: 406: 401: 400: 399: 394: 389: 384: 379: 374: 369: 364: 354: 353: 352: 347: 342: 337: 332: 321: 318: 317: 314: 313: 310: 309: 304: 295: 290: 285: 280: 275: 270: 265: 260: 255: 250: 245: 240: 235: 230: 225: 220: 215: 210: 205: 200: 195: 189: 186: 185: 182: 181: 178: 177: 172: 167: 162: 157: 152: 147: 142: 137: 131: 128: 127: 124: 123: 121: 120: 115: 110: 104: 101: 100: 94: 93: 81: 80: 35: 33: 26: 17: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1816: 1805: 1802: 1800: 1797: 1796: 1794: 1783: 1779: 1775: 1771: 1766: 1763: 1760: 1757: 1753: 1749: 1745: 1741: 1737: 1733: 1729: 1724: 1720: 1716: 1712: 1708: 1704: 1700: 1695: 1691: 1687: 1683: 1679: 1675: 1671: 1666: 1662: 1658: 1654: 1650: 1646: 1640: 1636: 1632: 1628: 1624: 1620: 1616: 1609: 1605: 1601: 1597: 1593: 1589: 1585: 1581: 1577: 1572: 1568: 1564: 1561:(1): 73–102. 1560: 1556: 1552: 1547: 1546: 1534: 1530: 1526: 1522: 1515: 1513: 1503: 1501: 1493: 1489: 1485: 1481: 1478: 1474: 1468: 1460: 1454: 1450: 1443: 1434: 1425: 1415: 1408: 1402: 1394: 1390: 1386: 1382: 1378: 1374: 1367: 1361:Wilcox (2004) 1358: 1349: 1341: 1337: 1333: 1329: 1325: 1321: 1317: 1313: 1306: 1299:(3): 613–619. 1298: 1294: 1287: 1285: 1283: 1274: 1268: 1264: 1260: 1255: 1247: 1239: 1237:9780809022816 1233: 1229: 1222: 1213: 1206: 1202: 1201: 1196: 1191: 1184: 1178: 1174: 1164: 1161: 1159: 1158:Sign relation 1156: 1154: 1151: 1149: 1146: 1144: 1141: 1139: 1136: 1134: 1131: 1129: 1126: 1125: 1118: 1116: 1112: 1108: 1103: 1100: 1095: 1091: 1074: 1066: 1058: 1046: 1034: 1032: 1027: 1021: 1017: 1006: 1004: 999: 995: 991: 987: 983: 979: 975: 972:), or of its 971: 967: 966: 962: 958: 954: 950: 947: 943: 939: 935: 925: 920: 918: 914: 903: 901: 897: 893: 892:arbitrariness 889: 883: 867: 865: 861: 856: 854: 850: 846: 836: 834: 830: 826: 822: 818: 814: 810: 806: 795: 793: 790: 786: 785:morphological 782: 778: 774: 769: 767: 762: 758: 753: 750: 746: 745:Winfried NĂśth 737: 734: 730: 729: 723: 721: 717: 712: 710: 706: 702: 698: 689: 687: 683: 679: 676: 672: 668: 664: 659: 657: 655: 653: 645: 643: 637: 632: 628: 627:reduplication 614: 611: 608: 607: 606: 603: 601: 597: 593: 589: 588:structuralist 585: 584:arbitrariness 581: 577: 573: 569: 565: 561: 557: 545: 540: 538: 533: 531: 526: 525: 523: 522: 519: 515: 511: 510: 503: 500: 498: 495: 493: 490: 488: 485: 483: 480: 478: 475: 473: 470: 468: 465: 463: 460: 458: 455: 453: 450: 448: 445: 443: 440: 438: 437:Descriptivism 435: 433: 430: 428: 425: 423: 420: 419: 413: 412: 405: 404:Structuralism 402: 398: 395: 393: 390: 388: 387:Prague circle 385: 383: 380: 378: 375: 373: 370: 368: 365: 363: 360: 359: 358: 355: 351: 348: 346: 343: 341: 338: 336: 333: 331: 328: 327: 326: 323: 322: 316: 315: 308: 305: 303: 299: 296: 294: 291: 289: 286: 284: 281: 279: 276: 274: 271: 269: 266: 264: 261: 259: 256: 254: 251: 249: 246: 244: 241: 239: 236: 234: 233:Documentation 231: 229: 226: 224: 221: 219: 216: 214: 211: 209: 208:Computational 206: 204: 201: 199: 196: 194: 191: 190: 184: 183: 176: 173: 171: 168: 166: 163: 161: 158: 156: 153: 151: 148: 146: 143: 141: 138: 136: 133: 132: 126: 125: 119: 116: 114: 111: 109: 106: 105: 103: 102: 99: 96: 95: 91: 87: 86: 77: 74: 66: 63:November 2015 56: 52: 46: 45: 39: 34: 25: 24: 21: 16: 1773: 1769: 1755: 1731: 1727: 1702: 1698: 1673: 1669: 1626: 1579: 1575: 1558: 1554: 1543:Bibliography 1524: 1520: 1506:Croft (1978) 1488:Tope Omoniyi 1486:, edited by 1483: 1467: 1458:0-80912298-7 1448: 1442: 1433: 1424: 1414: 1401: 1376: 1372: 1366: 1357: 1348: 1315: 1311: 1305: 1296: 1292: 1258: 1246: 1227: 1221: 1212: 1204: 1198: 1190: 1182: 1177: 1104: 1096: 1040: 1014:example, in 1012: 1002: 997: 989: 977: 963: 956: 948: 941: 931: 921: 909: 887: 884: 868: 857: 853:onomatopoeia 842: 820: 801: 770: 757:intertextual 754: 743: 726: 724: 713: 709:displacement 695: 682:Onomatopoeia 680: 660: 651: 647: 639: 631:Edward Sapir 623: 604: 563: 553: 446: 350:Glossematics 330:Constituency 302:interpreting 140:Lexicography 69: 60: 41: 20: 15: 1604:Davis, Marc 986:Paul Wexler 984:1955: 609, 976:adaptation 817:chimpanzees 787:pattern of 502:Terminology 477:Orthography 397:Usage-based 298:Translating 193:Acquisition 98:Linguistics 55:introducing 1793:Categories 1661:5706745444 1475:(2006), "' 1170:References 1107:morphology 829:Bee dances 809:ideophones 777:consonants 675:Haspelmath 671:biological 596:generative 568:similarity 472:Orismology 357:Functional 345:Generative 335:Dependency 155:Pragmatics 145:Morphology 135:Diachronic 38:references 1804:Semiotics 1340:147082731 1148:Semiotics 1003:Ursprache 789:adjective 766:allusions 749:signifier 720:megafauna 705:indexical 667:cognitive 642:täsäbbärä 592:formalist 564:iconicity 560:semiotics 447:Iconicity 442:Etymology 362:Cognitive 325:Formalist 278:Phonetics 268:Philology 160:Semantics 150:Phonology 1728:Language 1699:Language 1670:Language 1653:95-12073 1606:(1995). 1480:Othering 1373:Language 1138:Gematria 1121:See also 1115:Jakobson 1111:phonemes 1031:mnemonic 1016:Egyptian 888:computer 805:language 781:plosives 773:Phonemes 761:morpheme 714:Using a 638:, where 248:Forensic 228:Distance 175:Typology 90:a series 88:Part of 1332:2743729 1197:(1999) 974:Yiddish 957:ra` dam 946:Yiddish 845:gesture 813:mammals 792:grading 686:mimesis 663:natural 636:Amharic 580:meaning 572:analogy 203:Applied 113:History 108:Outline 51:improve 1748:413373 1746:  1719:414448 1717:  1690:412894 1688:  1659:  1651:  1641:  1596:307673 1594:  1455:  1419:89–97. 1393:412894 1391:  1338:  1330:  1269:  1234:  1077:, and 1053:, and 998:ra dam 990:ra dam 970:Warsaw 961:Polish 955:רע דם 953:Hebrew 949:ra dam 942:ra dom 940:רע דם 906:Poetry 878:+ YOU 821:et al. 650:täsäbb 518:Portal 416:Topics 165:Syntax 40:, but 1744:JSTOR 1715:JSTOR 1686:JSTOR 1613:. In 1611:(PDF) 1592:JSTOR 1389:JSTOR 1336:S2CID 1328:JSTOR 1195:Croft 1099:Sapir 980:(see 978:rĂłdem 965:Radom 944:(cf. 874:+ YOU 825:Kanzi 764:like 684:(and 118:Index 1657:OCLC 1649:LCCN 1639:ISBN 1490:and 1453:ISBN 1267:ISBN 1232:ISBN 1153:Sign 1051:/eÉŞ/ 669:and 594:and 576:sign 300:and 293:Text 1778:doi 1736:doi 1707:doi 1678:doi 1631:doi 1584:doi 1563:doi 1529:doi 1381:doi 1320:doi 1254:"1" 1105:In 1026:rem 1020:mer 1005:." 779:or 725:In 654:ärä 570:or 1795:: 1774:15 1772:. 1742:. 1732:59 1730:. 1713:. 1703:56 1701:. 1684:. 1674:51 1672:. 1655:. 1647:. 1637:. 1621:; 1617:; 1590:. 1580:22 1578:. 1559:27 1557:. 1553:. 1525:28 1523:. 1511:^ 1499:^ 1387:. 1377:51 1375:. 1334:. 1326:. 1316:34 1314:. 1297:35 1295:. 1281:^ 1265:. 1263:22 1257:. 1113:. 1087:uː 1069:, 1065:iː 1049:, 835:. 731:, 652:ab 602:. 590:, 562:, 92:on 1784:. 1780:: 1750:. 1738:: 1721:. 1709:: 1692:. 1680:: 1663:. 1633:: 1598:. 1586:: 1569:. 1565:: 1535:. 1531:: 1461:. 1409:. 1395:. 1383:: 1342:. 1322:: 1275:. 1240:. 1207:. 1090:/ 1084:j 1081:/ 1075:/ 1073:ʊ 1071:/ 1067:/ 1063:/ 1059:/ 1057:ĂŚ 1055:/ 1047:/ 1045:ɒ 1043:/ 880:n 876:2 872:1 543:e 536:t 529:v 76:) 70:( 65:) 61:( 47:.

Index

references
inline citations
improve
introducing
Learn how and when to remove this message
a series
Linguistics
Outline
History
Index
Diachronic
Lexicography
Morphology
Phonology
Pragmatics
Semantics
Syntax
Syntax–semantics interface
Typology
Acquisition
Anthropological
Applied
Computational
Conversation analysis
Corpus linguistics
Discourse analysis
Distance
Documentation
Ethnography of communication
Ethnomethodology

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑